Defecting from the gutenberg legacy; employing....pdf

Journal of Communication ISSN 0021-9916

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Defecting From the Gutenberg Legacy:
Employing Images to Test Knowledge Gaps
Maria Elizabeth Grabe, Ozen Bas, & Irene Ingeborg van Driel
The Media School, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405, USA

Education-based knowledge gaps are well-documented across countries, media platforms,
and content. Without exception, knowledge is measured through words not images. Given
the centrality of sight in the natural history of Homo sapiens, the extraordinary visual acuity of humans, and the proliferation of screen-based visual media environments in contemporary life, an experiment was conducted to test the knowledge gap visually. Participants
watched 8 audiovisual news stories. Simple recognition of story details and comprehension of that information were tested in verbal and visual modalities. Results offered the
first confirmation of the knowledge gap in visual terms. Yet, gaps were significantly smaller
employing visual than verbal measures, pointing to the need for continued efforts to develop
visual measures for future memory studies.
Keywords: Knowledge Gap, Information Processing, Images, Experiment, Memory, Visual
Processing.
doi:10.1111/jcom.12150

Over the past 40-plus years more than 200 studies have documented a persistent

education-based information acquisition divide called the knowledge gap. Despite
the robust findings, a number of scholars are growing uneasy about existing conceptualizations and measures of knowledge in contemporary knowledge gap research.
Some of this contrariety is spurred by cognitive science evidence of visual processing
as a potent mode of learning. Yet invariably, knowledge gaps have been measured
through verbal tests. Visual memory and comprehension measures have not been
employed, because researchers seem reluctant to operationalize knowledge in visual
terms. Doris Graber (2001) referred to this research orientation as The Gutenberg
Legacy, in which the written word is canonized as a conduit of information important for participatory citizenship. Images, by contrast, are treated as lacking serious
information value—perhaps because they are inherently entangled with emotion by
being processed via affective pathways in the brain. Dating back at least as far as the

Corresponding author: Maria Elizabeth Grabe; e-mail: mgrabe@indiana.edu
300

Journal of Communication 65 (2015) 300–319 © 2015 International Communication Association

M. E. Grabe et al.

Testing the Knowledge Gap Visually


Enlightenment, emotion is seen as irreconcilable with logic and reason. This tradition still anchors most contemporary predictive models of knowledge acquisition and
active citizenship (Marcus, Neuman, & MacKuen, 2000; Somit & Peterson, 1998) in
the imperative of rational choice, enabled by verbal language.
The importance of verbal language in the cultural history of Homo sapiens stands
beyond question. Yet, in the panoramic scope of evolutionary adaptation the human
brain has had relatively little time to specialize for oral (40,000 to 50,000 years; see
Holden, 2004) and written communication (about 5,000 years; see Houston, 2004).
In comparison, visual acuity is not an acquired skill or literacy—it is a vital sense,
capacitated by neurological adaptations over millions of years, that affords contemporary human cognition and behavior (Damasio, 1994; Gazzaniga, 1998; Parker, 2003).
The idea that visual acuity drove the evolutionary course that produced Homo sapiens
might appear lionized and distant as justification for contemporary media research.
Yet media use patterns are transformed by rapidly unfolding and sweeping shifts from
written words to audiovisuals. According to Cisco (2014), mobile traffic has increased
81% in 2013 and the forecast is that in 2018 video use will make up 69% of all global
mobile data traffic.
The signs are clear. A post-Gutenberg era has sprung. Screen-based media environments with their life-like full motion images (entangled with ambient sound and
speech) are likely to become as central as the printed word has been for the past five
centuries. Testing information acquisition through user-preferred modalities made
good research sense in a newspaper age; scholarly focus on the visual modality is
urgently necessary now. The experimental work reported here makes an effort to

advance a media research trajectory that recognizes the human acuity for visual
processing and growing preference for using media in that modality. Specifically, we
(re)tested the knowledge gap, a 4-decade-old theory built on verbal assessments of
information gain. In doing so, we employed visual and verbal measures comparatively
to understand two different layers of knowledge acquisition: encoding (recognition
memory) and comprehension of information.
Verbal knowledge gaps

Since the mid-1970s, more than 200 studies have consistently documented differential
knowledge gain across education or socioeconomic status (SES) groups (see Gaziano,
1997, 2010; Hwang & Jeong, 2009). Evidence is robust across cultural contexts (Bonfadelli, 2002; Hwang & Jeong, 2009; Nguyen & Western, 2007; Van Dijk & Hacker,
2003; Yang & Grabe, 2011), media platforms (Grabe, Kamhawi, & Yegiyan, 2009; Kim,
2008; Wei & Hindman, 2011), and content (Ettema, Brown, & Luepker, 1983; Grabe,
Yegiyan, & Kamhawi, 2008; Jerit, Barabas, & Bolsen, 2006). Moreover, individual differences among audience members have been tested to explain the gap (Eveland &
Scheufele, 2000; Kwak, 1999; Neuman, Just, & Crigler, 1992; Price & Zaller, 1993).
Knowledge gap research also moved from exclusively survey to experimental
methodologies. Based on the mechanics of the limited capacity model (Craik &
Journal of Communication 65 (2015) 300–319 © 2015 International Communication Association

301


Testing the Knowledge Gap Visually

M. E. Grabe et al.

Lockhart, 1972; Shiffrin & Schneider, 1977; Tulving & Thompson, 1973) of information processing (applied to media—see Lang, 2000), the knowledge gap (or news
learning studies) tested memory formation at encoding, storage, and retrieval levels.
Yet, most studies focused on encoding, using recognition tests (Grabe, Lang, Zhou, &
Bolls, 2000; Grabe et al., 2009; Shoemaker, Schooler, & Danielson, 1989; Tremayne &
Dunwoody, 2001) and confirmed that lower education groups processed (encoded)
news information less efficiently than higher education groups (Grabe et al., 2000,
2008, 2009; Yang & Grabe, 2011). As a departure point of connection to survey and
experimental evidence on the knowledge gap, we hypothesize that people with higher
levels of education will outperform those with lower levels of education in verbal
recognition tests of news information (H1).
A number of scholars have, however, grown uneasy about conceptualizing,
quantifying, and measuring knowledge (Bonfadelli, 2002; Gaziano, 1997; Prior, 2014;
Selwyn, 2004; Woodall, Davis, & Sahin, 1983) as encoding (recognition memory)
rather than comprehension of news (Eveland, Marton, & Seo, 2004; Findahl &
Höijer, 1985; Gaziano, 1997; 2010; Graber, 1988; Hindman, 2009; Hwang & Jeong,

2009; Robinson & Levy, 1986; Snoeijer, de Vreese, & Semetko, 2002; Woodall et al.,
1983). The traditional view of informed citizenship as memory for cold hard facts is
challenged by reconceptualizations of informed citizenship as applied understanding,
comprehension, and even emotional involvement with social issues.
Information processing scholars treat comprehension and memory as two distinct cognitive processes (Ortony, 1978). Comprehension measures involve more than
memory for discrete information and are more often used among researchers who do
work on learning from news media (e.g., Robinson & Levy, 1986; Woodall et al., 1983)
than those who work in the limited capacity tradition (Gunter, 1987; Lang, Zhou,
Schwartz, Bolls, & Potter, 2000). Comprehension measures assess if participants have
integrated information into a meaningful system of existing knowledge (Robinson
& Levy, 1986; Tremayne & Dunwoody, 2001). While measures vary across studies,
comprehension tests are centrally concerned with how well participants grasp the
gist of a story or can apply acquired information in other contexts. These outcomes
are arguably more conducive to the ideals of informed citizenship than remembering peripheral details like the names of people and places. Memory for details from
a story is not seen as evidence that the audience understood what the story is about.
At the same time, comprehension of the story does not necessarily assure memory of
factual details contained in it (Graber, 1988). Yet, correlations between memory and
comprehension are often reported (Basil, 1994; Edwardson, Kent, Engstrom, & Hofmann, 1992; Findahl & Höijer, 1985) and high education groups have been shown
to verbally recount and comprehend material from text-based media at higher rates
than low education groups (Grabe et al., 2009; Yang & Grabe, 2011). Given existing

evidence—and in line with the preceding hypothesis for recognition memory—it is
expected that the knowledge gap will persist in verbal comprehension test scores, such
that people with higher levels of education will outscore those with lower levels of
education (H2).
302

Journal of Communication 65 (2015) 300–319 © 2015 International Communication Association

M. E. Grabe et al.

Testing the Knowledge Gap Visually

Visual primacy and the knowledge gap

In evolutionary circles explanations for why the human brain is better adapted for
visual than verbal information processing are often based on the relatively short history of verbal language in the natural history of Homo sapiens. In this regard, Paivio
(1971) argues that words are processed in a linear fashion—one at a time—while
visuals are processed faster and holistically as an entanglement of verbal and visual
memory records. When one of these routes fails at the retrieval stage, the other often
can successfully manage the task (Paivio, 1971). This acknowledgement of visual primacy in Homo sapiens (and other primates) has unwrinkled the path for ontological

shifts in cognitive and neuroscience thinking about the enmeshment of rationality
and emotion.
In Descartes’ Error, Damasio (1994) argued on the basis of neurological evidence
that humans are not primarily thinking beings who also feel, but feeling beings
who also think. Visual processing is singled out as the leading conduit of emotional
involvement and the dominant mode of learning and building synaptic connections
(Barry, 2005; Damasio, 1994). Yet, knowledge gain has been consistently measured
through verbal means. Visual memory and comprehension instruments are strikingly underdeveloped because knowledge is not operationalized in visual terms. “The
belief that audiovisuals are poor carriers of important information has become so
ingrained in conventional wisdom,” Graber (2001) observes, “that it has throttled
research” (p. 93). Her calls (Graber, 1988, 1990, 1996, 2001) for revising the way
media and political communication scholars think about the importance of images
as information sources have not been answered earnestly, which instigates the next
hypothesis. Regardless of education level, recognition and comprehension scores will
be higher when measured visually than verbally (H3).
Some notable exceptions to The Gutenberg Legacy are experimental studies
examining the effects of candidate facial emotion on voter evaluations (Keating,
Randall, & Kendrick, 1999; Rosenberg, Kahn, & Tran, 1991), Graber’s (1988, 1990,
2001) pioneering work on creating visual measures, and Prior’s (2014) effort to establish visual political knowledge measures. In a series of studies, Graber (2001) found
that viewers recalled more visual than verbal themes from television. Prior (2014)

employed visual (faces) and verbal (names) recognition questions to get to the encoding process of political knowledge. For example, a question like “Who is the current
Senate Majority Leader?” represented a verbal prompt and featured four verbal
(name) options. The visual version of the question featured the same verbal prompt
but offered four visual representations of faces. Prior (2014) found that differences
among college graduates and nongraduates declined significantly when knowledge
was measured visually. This suggests that the potential of images as a knowledge
equalizer might be unrealized due to the yardstick used to gauge knowledge (Prior,
2014). Indeed, the low levels of knowledge routinely reported in survey research and
learning from news studies might partially be due to the kind of knowledge measures
that are employed, and the circular logic that underlies them. For example, education
Journal of Communication 65 (2015) 300–319 © 2015 International Communication Association

303

Testing the Knowledge Gap Visually

M. E. Grabe et al.

level has been linked to political knowledge which is measured through the written
word, that requires literacy. It thus seems unsurprising that political knowledge and

comprehension of political complexities—measured through the written word—are
linked to formal education (Jennings, 1996; Nie, Junn, & Stehlik-Barry, 1996). Graber
(1996) interprets these findings as a matter of literacy in aid of verbal processing. We
argue that literacy also serves verbal knowledge measures. On the other hand, visual
information processing requires no literacy, develops practically at birth, and enables
most citizens to learn about their social world from a very young age. For unclear
reasons, visual processing and visual measures of knowledge remain customarily idle
in scholarly pursuits to understand informed citizenship.
It is noteworthy that when Tichenor, Donohue, and Olien (1970) first formulated the knowledge gap hypothesis, they suggested that audiovisual learning might
enhance knowledge levels among less literate groups: “Since television use tends to be
less correlated with education, there is a possibility that television may be a ‘knowledge
leveler’ in some areas. Whether TV does in fact have such a leveling function seems
to be an urgent matter for further research” (p. 170). More than 40 years later, there is
no consensus about the potential of audiovisual learning as an equalizer across education groups. Some studies showed that visuals aid (Grabe et al., 2009; Gunter, 1987;
Katz, Adoni, & Parness, 1977; McCarthy & Warrington, 1988) while others found that
they inhibited memory especially when news stories contained discrepant information across visuals and verbals (Brosius, Donsbach, & Birk, 1996; Van der Molen &
Klijn, 2004; Zhou, 2005) or when they were highly compelling (Grabe, Lang, & Zhao,
2003; Miller & Leshner, 2007; Newhagen & Reeves, 1992). Yet, these studies did not
measure memory visually. They mostly tested the effects of audiovisual content using
verbal information measures or, as Graber (2001) points out, “In tests of what viewers

learn from television news, scholars have usually asked only for recall of the verbal
message, assuming that the visuals mirrored the verbal texts or conveyed no significant substantive information at all” (p. 93). Thus, because visuals are not considered to
have much information value, they are not used as an index of consequential processes
such as memory formation or comprehension of social issues (Graber, 1990).
Indeed, most existing knowledge tests do not measure image-based recognition
and comprehension. If cognitive scientists are right in arguing that Homo sapiens
is generously endowed with visual information processing abilities—compared
to verbal processing, which is a recent environmentally driven adaptation—the
knowledge gap between higher and lower education groups might disappear if visual
knowledge measures are employed. Grounded in this line of thinking, but lacking
evidence for formal hypotheses, a research question (RQ1) prompts the comparison
of education-based knowledge gaps measured through visual versus verbal modalities
for both recognition and comprehension measures.
The time factor

Since Bartlett’s (1932) seminal study on memory decay, the idea that memory records
become less vivid and accurate over time has enjoyed substantial research attention.
304

Journal of Communication 65 (2015) 300–319 © 2015 International Communication Association


M. E. Grabe et al.

Testing the Knowledge Gap Visually

Demographic factors such as education, age, and gender as well as the type of memory
(episodic, source, spatial) and stimuli characteristics (emotional dimensions, faces,
digits, word-pairs) have been tested for influencing memory decay. Few of these studies have employed stimuli that resemble news.
Dixon, Simon, Nowak, and Hultsch (1982) tested the effect of a week delay on
three adult groups and found that although younger participants recalled text materials better than middle-aged and older participants, memory decreased over time for
all three age groups. Meeter, Murre, and Janssen (2005) investigated memory for news
among 14,000 participants worldwide asking questions about news that ranged from
as recent as 2 days to 2 years old. Over time, participants forget information regardless
of their initial learning rate. Some evidence points to education as inhibiting forgetting: the higher the education level, the slower the memory decay (Karrasch & Laine,
2003; Morrow & Ryan, 2002). In a knowledge gap study that employed a 48-hour time
delay in measures of encoding, storage, and retrieval, Grabe et al. (2009) found evidence of greater memory declines for the low than high education group, across all
three measures. With a week long delay, Bas and Grabe (2013) found that low education participants had stronger memory decay than higher educated participants.
This offers some point of traction for a hypothesis (H4) predicting that, regardless of
modality, the low education group will be associated with more recognition memory
and comprehension decay than the higher education group.
Given the sparseness of evidence related to the interaction of audience education
level, memory test modality, and time delay, the last research question (RQ2) asks for
a comparison of modalities and participant education levels for each of the memory
measures (recognition and comprehension).
Method
Design

This experiment employed a mixed factorial 2 (participant education level) × 2 (test
modality) × 2 (time) model. Education level (high and low) was a between-subjects
factor while modality (visual or verbal) and time (two times of measurement 1 week
apart) were treated as within-subjects factors. The different levels of modality were
represented by verbal and visual memory measures. Four random story sequences
were constructed to control for the effects of order.
Participants

Eighty subjects participated in this study conducted in a small Midwestern city in the
United States. Low (N = 40) and high (N = 40) education participants were recruited
from the local university community, neighborhood e-mail distribution groups,
Craigslist.com, and local charity organizations. A number of screening questions
about age and education were used to select subjects. The high education group
participants had all earned at least an M.A. degree. Participants in the low education
group held no more than 2 years of vocational training. In order to control for
Journal of Communication 65 (2015) 300–319 © 2015 International Communication Association

305

Testing the Knowledge Gap Visually

M. E. Grabe et al.

potential gender effects, equal numbers of men and women were recruited for the
two education groups. The lower age limit was set at 25 to allow time for education to
unfold while the upper age limit was fixed at 55 years. The average age of participants
in this study was 34 (high education = 32; low education = 36). Two participants in
the low education group and seven in the high education group self-identified as
ethnic minorities. An a priori power analysis revealed that with 76 subjects, effect
sizes of .25 should be detected with .95 power in this experimental design. With 80
participants, this study was adequately powered to detect effects.
Stimuli

Eight news stories were selected from a pool of 26 ABC news stories reported by Brian
Ross. His investigative reporting style is marked by the inclusion of factual information linked to related contextual issues. For example, factual information about sexual
harassment of teenagers in the workplace was tied to the context of employees who
are uninformed about their rights and managers who receive little training on professional conduct. All stories aired on television and became available on the ABC news
website, some with additional material. They did not contain negatively compelling
images as operationalized by Newhagen and Reeves (1992). The civic importance of
the story issue and its potential to resonate across gender and socioeconomic groups
were used as guidelines for selection. For example, a story about illegal trade of Chinese prisoner body parts was excluded based on the argument that its international
scope might not draw similar levels of interest and identification from all participants.
The chosen eight stories were about (a) corruption in higher administration of Federal
Public Housing, (b) illegal immigrant child labor in agriculture, (c) sleep-deprivation
among airline pilots, (d) malfunctioning brakes on Toyota cars, (e) abusive and illegal
debt collection practices, (f) sexual harassment of teenage employees in the fast food
industry, (g) animal abuse in milk production, (h) lethal but legal drugs.
Dependent variables

Memory was assessed using five-option multiple choice questions, at two points, 1
week apart. Simple verbal recognition questions featured a verbal prompt and verbal answer options whereas visual recognition questions had a verbal prompt with
visual options to choose from (see Figure 1 for sample questions). Simple factual
recognition measurement, as is often done in studies of this kind, targeted familiarity
with the material and featured classic encoding measures. Each participant answered
a total of 64 simple recognition questions—16 in each modality (32 total) at each
time point (64 in total). Because simple recognition tests are criticized for not assessing depth in understanding of issues, the study reported here developed questions
with the aim to get to comprehension. Applied knowledge or comprehension was
measured with one visual and one verbal question, per story, per time point, adding
up to another 32 questions in total—eight visual, eight verbal at each time point of
measurement. These questions required reasoning and called for application of the
verbal and visual material presented in the experimental stimuli. For example, simple
306

Journal of Communication 65 (2015) 300–319 © 2015 International Communication Association

M. E. Grabe et al.

Simple recognition

Testing the Knowledge Gap Visually

Comprehension

Figure 1 Question examples of visual (above) and verbal (below) version of simple recognition and comprehension.

recognition (visual and verbal) of who regulates debt collection is enough for a correct
answer, as shown in Figure 1. Comprehension, on the other hand, required not only
remembering information given in the story (i.e., advice to consumers on how to protect themselves against collector abuse) but also application of that information in
selecting an answer. Verbal and visual questions for recognition and comprehension
were designed so that content and difficulty levels were comparable across modalities.
Stimuli did not include negatively compelling images and therefore the memory and
comprehension tests were also void of such material. Still images of people (serving
coffee, walking on the street, in interview), objects (blueberries, tape recorder, cars),
and places (airport, street scenes, home interior) were used.
Procedure

Data were collected from one participant at a time. Each was greeted at the experimental room that resembled a home/work office and then asked to take a seat
at a desktop computer. Here a participant received information and instructions
about the data collection activities. When they were ready, the viewing process was
activated by the participant. The stories were presented on a computer screen with
Journal of Communication 65 (2015) 300–319 © 2015 International Communication Association

307

Testing the Knowledge Gap Visually

M. E. Grabe et al.

a simple advance button to move linearly through the eight stories. After each story,
participants answered evaluative questions. These and demographic and media use
items (administered after viewing was completed) served as memory distractors.
Memory and comprehension measures were completed next. Participants were
thanked and scheduled for the second data collection a week later. There were no
stimuli to watch at the second time point. Similar procedures were followed to collect
memory data at Time 2.
Findings

Mixed (between- and within-subjects) ANOVA (analysis of variance) procedures
were conducted on simple recognition and comprehension measures: 2 (education level of participants: higher and lower) × 2 (test modality: visual and verbal
measures) × 2 (time: immediately after exposure and a week later). Results are summarized in Table 1. Post hoc paired comparisons were done to identify the critical
cells in significant models.
Testing the knowledge gap across the board

The first and second hypotheses required testing main effects for education. Knowledge gaps were confirmed for both verbal simple recognition and verbal comprehension measures. Higher educated participants (M = 5.96; SE = .19) had higher verbal
simple recognition scores than lower educated participants (M = 3.79; SE = .19). Similarly, verbal comprehension scores were significantly higher for the high (M = 3.70;
SE = .12) than low (M = 2.11; SE = .12) education group.
Testing the impact of modality

Hypothesis 3 made a forecast for modality main effects on both recognition and
comprehension tests such that visual tests would produce higher scores than verbal
tests. This hypothesis was also supported. As Table 1 shows, there was a significant
main effect in the predicted direction for test modality on simple recognition (visual:
M = 6.04, SE = .12; verbal: M = 4.88, SE = .13) and comprehension (visual: M = 3.15,
SE = .08; verbal: M = 2.91, SE = .08).
Tests at the crossing of education and modality

RQ1 prompted a sequence of tests to assess the influence of different question
modalities on knowledge acquisition across education groups. In short, there were
significant interaction effects for modality and education on recognition and comprehension, as depicted in Figures 2 and 3. Overall, the low education group was
more sensitive to modality differences. Post hoc paired comparisons confirmed
that asking questions visually narrowed the knowledge gap. Yet, there was a persistent gap between education groups on both visual (low education: M = 5.39,
SE = .16; high education: M = 6.69, SE = .16), t(78) = −5.50, p = .001, and verbal (low
education: M = 3.79, SE = .19; high education: M = 5.96, SE = .19); t(78) = −8.02,
308

Journal of Communication 65 (2015) 300–319 © 2015 International Communication Association

M. E. Grabe et al.

Testing the Knowledge Gap Visually

Table 1 F-Test Results for Analysis of Variance on Simple Recognition and Comprehension
Measures
Variables

F

p

η2

Verbal measures only
Simple recognition:
Main Effect, Education (H1)
Comprehension:
Main Effect, Education (H2)

64.47

.001

.452

92.230

.001

.542

Simple recognition—full model
Main Effect, Education
Main Effect, Modality (H3)
Education × Modality (RQ1)
Main Effect, Time
Time × Education (H4)
Time × Modality
Time × Education × Modality (RQ2)

61.11
91.23
12.96
32.69
1.59
.27
.002

.001
.001
.001
.001
.211
.605
.962

.443
.195
.028
.075
.004
.001
.000

Comprehension—full model
Main Effect, Education
Main Effect, Modality (H3)
Education × Modality (RQ1)
Main Effect, Time
Time × Education (H4)
Time × Modality
Time × Education × Modality (RQ2)

72.88
13.77
27.81
3.25
3.25
8.55
.64

.001
.001
.001
.075
.075
.005
.428

.487
.025
.050
.014
.014
.008
.001

Note: df = 1.

p = .001, recognition measures. Yet, from Figure 2 it is clear that the gap narrowed on
visual measures. Moreover, both education groups, low: t(39) = 7.40, p = .001; high:
t(39) = 5.70, p = .001, scored significantly higher in visual, low: M = 5.39, SE = .16;
high: M = 6.69, SE = .16, than verbal, low: M = 3.79, SE = .19; high: M = 5.96,
SE = .19, recognition.
Figure 3 shows that comprehension data followed the trend of simple recognition. The knowledge gap was smaller for the visual than verbal measures, yet large
enough to be statistically significant for both modalities: t(78) = −6.01, p = .001 and
t(78) = −9.60, p = .001, respectively. Strikingly, while the low education group performed significantly better on visual (M = 2.69, SE = .11) than on verbal (M = 2.12,
SE = .12) comprehension questions: t(39) = 5.00, p = .001, higher-educated people
did not differ significantly in their comprehension of news across the two modalities:
visual: M = 3.60, SE = .11; verbal: M = 3.70, SE = .12, t(39) = −1.87, p = .069.
Testing the time factor

Because lasting knowledge of social issues is conceptually linked to the idea of
informed citizenship, the fourth hypothesis tested for time influences on memory
Journal of Communication 65 (2015) 300–319 © 2015 International Communication Association

309

M. E. Grabe et al.

Testing the Knowledge Gap Visually

8

Mean: Simple recognition

7
6
5
4
3
2
1
0
Visual

Verbal
Modality

Low Education

High Education

Figure 2 Interaction effect for education level of participant and modality on simple recognition.

formation of news. The low education group was expected to show more simple
recognition and comprehension memory decay than the high education group.
There were main effects for time on simple recognition measures (see Table 1)
such that memory decay significantly progressed from Time 1 (M = 5.83, SE = .13) to
a week later, at Time 2 (M = 5.10, SE = .12). Comprehension faded less dramatically
over time (Time 1: M = 3.12, SE = .08; Time 2: M = 2.94, SE = .09), indicated by the
main effect that only approached significance (see Table 1). It appears that once information is comprehended, as opposed to being simply encoded, it is less vulnerable to
decay.
The fourth hypothesis, predicting a Time × Education interaction effect, found
sparse support in this data set (see Table 1). The Time × Education interaction
on simple recognition was not significant and it only approached significance on
comprehension. Thus, recognition memory for simple facts, regardless of modality,
decayed across education groups at a similar rate. Yet, comprehension decayed faster
for the low education group. In fact, post hoc paired comparisons showed that the
low education group was comprehending significantly better at Time 1 (M = 2.59,
SE = .12) than Time 2 (M = 2.23, SE = .13), t(39) = 2.07, p = .045. The high education
group’s comprehension held at exactly the same level (M = 3.65, SE = .13) over
time (t < 1). This is graphically represented in Figure 4. Also noteworthy is that the
310

Journal of Communication 65 (2015) 300–319 © 2015 International Communication Association

M. E. Grabe et al.

Testing the Knowledge Gap Visually

4
Mean: Comprehension

3.5
3
2.5
2
1.5
1
0.5
0
Visual

Verbal
Modality

Low Education

High Education

Figure 3 Interaction effect for education level of participant and modality on comprehension.

comprehension gap was significant at both time points: Time 1, t(78) = −6.42,
p = .001 and Time 2, t(78) = −7.74, p = .001.
Although there were no predictions for Time × Modality interaction effects, a
significant one (on comprehension) emerged from the ANOVA procedures used here
to test hypotheses (see Table 1 and Figure 5). Regardless of participant education
level, visual comprehension decayed less over time than verbal comprehension.

Mean: Comprehension

4
3.5
3
2.5
2
1.5
1
0.5
0
Time 1

Time 2
Time

Low Education

High Education

Figure 4 Interaction effect (approaching significance) for education level of participant and
time on comprehension.

Journal of Communication 65 (2015) 300–319 © 2015 International Communication Association

311

M. E. Grabe et al.

Testing the Knowledge Gap Visually

Mean: Comprehension

3.5
3
2.5
2
1.5
1
0.5
0
Time 1

Time 2
Time
Visual

Verbal

Figure 5 Interaction effect for time and modality on comprehension.

Post hoc paired comparisons of cells showed that there was a significant difference,
t(79) = 3.94, p = .001, between visual (M = 3.13, SE = .09) and verbal (M = 2.75,
SE = .12) comprehension at Time 2 and also a statistically notable decay of verbal
comprehension (Time 1: M = 3.07, SE = .09; Time 2: M = 2.75, SE = .11) over time,
t(79) = 2.85, p = .006. No other cell comparisons produced significant differences.
RQ2 prompts testing for a three-way interaction of Education, Modality, and
Time to compare gap sizes across visual and verbal modalities, over time. Neither
the simple recognition nor comprehension three-way interactions were significant
(see Table 1). The absence of interactions on both knowledge measures suggests that
the intersection of independent variables is statistically unremarkable. Nonetheless,
secondary analyses and visualizing them in graphic form provided clues to latent
associations that might not surface in an overall interaction assessment. Figure 6
offers a side-by-side view of these results. The simple recognition graph illustrates
the parallel influence of time on memory decay for both education groups with
verbal testing producing lower scores than visual testing. Unsurprisingly, all post
hoc paired comparisons were statistically significant, as can be expected from previously reported main effects and two-way interactions. Thus, while there is plenty
of variance within levels of independent variables, it does not spill out in statistical
association across independent variables.
Secondary analyses for comprehension, dissimilarly to simple recognition,
showed reciprocal relationships across independent variables. From Figure 6 it is
clear that lines cross and diverge in unparallel patterns. The high education group’s
scores produced statistically sapless comparisons—they were practically immune to
variation in test modality over time. Low education participants, in stark contrast,
312

Journal of Communication 65 (2015) 300–319 © 2015 International Communication Association

Testing the Knowledge Gap Visually

8

4

7

3.5
Mean: Comprehension

Mean: Simple recognition

M. E. Grabe et al.

6
5
4
3
2
1

3
2.5
2
1.5
1
0.5

0

0
Visual

Verbal
Modality
Low Education Time 1
Low Education Time 2
High Education Time 1
High Education Time 2

Visual

Verbal
Modality
Low Education Time 1
Low Education Time 2
High Education Time 1
High Education Time 2

Figure 6 Three-way interaction effects for education level of participant, question modality,
and time on simple recognition and comprehension.

showed significant over time decay in verbal, t(39) = 2.279, p = .009, compared to
visual, t(39) = .977, p = .334, comprehension scores. To clarify this point, Figure 6
graphically shows how citizens with lower levels of education had diminished capacity in showcasing their comprehension of issues in verbal tests across time points.
Visual tests of comprehension did not produce a significant depreciation over time,
as is also clear from Figure 6.
Concluding thoughts

The results of this study offer the first known confirmation of the knowledge gap based
on comparable visual and verbal knowledge measures. At the same time, markers of
visual primacy also emerged from this data set. For example, there were robust main
effects for modality (verbal vs. visual) on both memory and comprehension tests.
Thus when traditionally used verbal recognition memory measures were matched by
visual counterparts, participants (regardless of education level) performed better in
visual than verbal tests. It is also clear that for comprehension, visual tests produced
less evidence of time decay than the equivalent verbal measures. These findings provide reason to be somewhat cautious about the size of knowledge gaps that have been
reported over more than 40 years of mostly survey research, based exclusively on verbal measures of knowledge. Indeed, education-based inequities in knowledge gain
showed up with more vigor when verbal than visual measures were used. It is important to keep in mind though that while information acquisition gaps shrunk across
test modalities, they did not disappear. Moreover, compared to education, modality
Journal of Communication 65 (2015) 300–319 © 2015 International Communication Association

313

Testing the Knowledge Gap Visually

M. E. Grabe et al.

main effect sizes were small. Thus, the knowledge gap remains robust regardless of
test modality and time delay. In that sense, Tichenor et al.’s (1970) knowledge gap
hypothesis gained another evidentiary foothold in this data set.
Althaus (2012) recommends our discipline would benefit from normative assessments that situate evaluative scholarly stances within larger debates. The inevitability
of audiovisual screen-based technology as the dominant future conduit of media use
prompted us to take stock of existing knowledge measures, stretching and adapting
them into visual instruments of measurement. Doing so strays from the dominant
research paradigm, grounded in enlightenment convictions about the supremacy of
rational thought, facilitated by words. Testing the knowledge gap visually aligns with
political scientists such as Doris Graber and Markus Prior who have pioneered the
idea that both visual and verbal modalities should be used to measure news retention
and knowledge (Graber, 1994, 1996; Prior, 2014). As Prior (2014) cautioned, however, it is not always possible to ask questions visually. In working to accomplish this
goal we discovered the extraordinary challenge of posing the same questions in two
modalities, which probably explains why researchers have avoided visual measures
for so long. Yet, as Figure 1 demonstrates, comparable measures for both modalities
testing simple recognition and applied conceptual knowledge (comprehension) can
be accomplished.
Moving beyond simple recognition measures generated important insights. A
growing number of scholars have noted that recognition memory tests might not offer
an assessment of knowledge that would capacitate informed citizenship. Compared
to other measures of information gain (e.g., simple recognition, cued recall, or free
recall), lasting comprehension is arguably the grandest of achievements in knowledge
acquisition. This cognitive accomplishment is also central to the traditional mission
of journalism: to inform citizens, across all demographic lines, for participatory
democratic practice.
In this data set, different patterns emerged for education groups on simple recognition memory and comprehension over time. While simple recognition memory
decayed at similar levels for both groups over the course of 1 week, comprehension
declined only in the low education group. This finding deserves further investigation
to find out if (a) comprehended material is more robust over time than encoded
details, once it is committed to memory and (b) if this information processing
aptitude is a byproduct of formal education. If the trend (education helps to retain
comprehension) holds up under further scrutiny, it might be indicative of a cognitive strategy not to exhaust finite cognitive means in the service of absorbing and
retaining simple details. Perhaps this shows a preference for deriving insights (e.g.,
what to do when a debt collector is abusive or judging who is responsible for illegal
child labor) on how to navigate a complicated social environment. Future research
is needed to test this differential in managing cognitive reserves. It might very well
be that citizens retain what is conducive to productive maneuvering through life
(comprehension) more than scattered details (simple recognition). Differential life
and occupational demands across high- and low-education groups might lead to
314

Journal of Communication 65 (2015) 300–319 © 2015 International Communication Association

M. E. Grabe et al.

Testing the Knowledge Gap Visually

different habitual cognitive strategies (e.g., automaticity, elaboration, analytical) that
serve optimal functioning in response to environmental demands. Clearly, different
memory measures produce rifts across education groups. Of particular interest is
the intersection of education, modality, and time delay. Lower education groups
show memory decay most clearly for verbal comprehension. Future research, with
adequate statistical power to detect three-way interactions, is needed to shed more
light on how images might cognitively capacitate people with lower education levels.
Moreover, field experiments and ethnographic work could advance insight into how
different education groups use media in an increasingly screen-based environment.
This study’s data set suggests that, in the longer term, high-education groups seem
better positioned to employ encoded information to the benefit of comprehension.
These information navigation skills are likely to perpetuate existing education-based
social and economic hierarchies. From a macro vantage point, incorporating media
literacy (with emphasis on visuality) in formal education curricula seems overdue.
This study offers an increment in slow-rising evidence that images should be
included in assessments of knowledge. The structural exclusion of citizens with
lower levels of formal education from experimental studies through the overuse of
convenient undergraduate subject pools is a matter of weak (but sometimes tolerable)
external validity in social science practice. What is untenable, however, is continued
verbal-only measures of how citizens acquire information from media. This tradition
engenders a type of measurement bias against citizens with lower levels of education
who have greater ability to exhibit what they encode and comprehend through
visual assessments. At the risk of overstatement: It is time for an amiable ontological
defection from The Gutenberg Legacy.
References
Althaus, S. L. (2012). What’s good and bad in political communication research? Normative
standards for evaluating media and citizen performance. In H. A. Sametko & M.
Scammell (Eds.), The Sage handbook of political communication (pp. 97–112). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Barry, A. (2005). Perception theory. In K. Smith, S. Moriarty, G. Barbatsis, & K. Kenney
(Eds.), Handbook of visual communication (pp. 45–62). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Bartlett, F. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. London,
England: Cambridge University Press.
Bas, O., & Grabe, M. E. (2013). Emotion-provoking personalization of news: Informing
citizens and closing the knowledge gap? Communication Research. Advance online
publication. doi:10.1177/0093650213514602
Basil, M. (1994). Empirical examination of modality-specific attention to television scenes.
Communication Research, 21(2), 208–231. doi:10.1177/009365094021002004.
Bonfadelli, H. (2002). The Internet and knowledge gaps. European Journal of Communication,
17(1), 65–84. doi:10.1177/0267323102017001607.
Brosius, H., Donsbach, W., & Birk, M. (1996). How do text-picture relations affect the
informational effectiveness of television newscasts? Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic
Media, 40(2), 180–195. doi:10.1080/08838159609364343.
Journal of Communication 65 (2015) 300–319 © 2015 International Communication Association

315

Testing the Knowledge Gap Visually

M. E. Grabe et al.

Cisco (2014, February 5). Cisco Visual Networking Index: Global Mobile Data Traffic Forecast
Update, 2013–2018. Retrieved from http://www.cisco.com/c/en/us/solutions/collateral/
service-provider/visual-networking-index-vni/white_paper_c11-520862.html
Craik, F., & Lockhart, R. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11(6), 671–684. doi:10.1016/S00225371(72)80001-X.
Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. New York, NY:
G. P. Putnam’s Sons.
Dixon, R., Simon, E., Nowak, C., & Hultsch, D. (1982). Text recall in adulthood as a function
of level of information, input modality, and delay interval. Journal of Gerontology, 37(3),
358–364. doi:10.1093/geronj/37.3.358.
Edwardson, M., Kent, K., Engstrom, E., & Hofmann, R. (1992). Audio recall immediately
following video change in television news. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media,
36(4), 395–410. doi:10.1080/08838159209364189.
Ettema, J., Brown, J., & Luepker, R. (1983). Knowledge gap effects in a health information
campaign. Public Opinion Quarterly, 47(4), 516–527. doi:10.1086/268809.
Eveland, W., Marton, K., & Seo, M. (2004). The influence of online news on the content and
structure of public affairs knowledge. Communication Research, 31(1), 82–108.
doi:10.1177/0093650203260203.
Eveland, W., & Scheufele, D. (2000). Connecting media use with gaps in knowledge and
participation. Political Communication, 17(3), 215–237. doi:10.1080/105846000414250.
Findahl, O., & Höijer, B. (1985). Some characteristics of news memory and comprehension.
Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 29(4), 379–396.
doi:10.1080/08838158509386594.
Gaziano, C. (1997). Forecast 2000: Widening knowledge gaps. Journalism and Mass
Communication Quarterly, 74(2), 237–264. doi:10.1177/107769909707400202.
Gaziano, C. (2010). Notes on “Revisiting the Knowledge Gap Hypothesis: A Meta-analysis of
Thirty-five Years of Research”. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 87(3–4),
615–632. doi:10.1177/107769901008700311.
Gazzaniga, M. (1998). The mind’s past. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Grabe, M. E., Kamhawi, R., & Yegiyan, N. (2009). Informing citizens: How people with
different levels of education process television, newspapers, and web news. Journal of
Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 53(1), 90–111. doi:10.1080/08838150802643860.
Grabe, M. E., Lang, A., & Zhao, X. (2003). News content and form implications for memory
and audience evaluations. Communication Research, 30(4), 387–413. doi:10.1177/00936
50203253368.
Grabe, M. E., Lang, A., Zhou, S., & Bolls, P. (2000). Cognitive access to negatively arousing
news: An experimental investigation of the knowledge gap. Communication Research,
27(1), 3–26. doi:10.1177/009365000027001001.
Grabe, M. E., Yegiyan, N., & Kamhawi, R. (2008). Experimental evidence of the knowledge
gap: Message arousal, motivation, and time delay. Human Communication Research,
34(4), 550–571. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2008.00332.x.
Graber, D. (1988). Processing the news: How people tame the information tide. White Plains,
NY: Longman.
Graber, D. (1990). Seeing is remembering: How visuals contribute to learning from television
news. Journal of Communication, 40(3), 134–155. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1990.
tb02275.x.
316

Journal of Communication 65 (2015) 300–319 © 2015 International Communication Association

M. E. Grabe et al.

Testing the Knowledge Gap Visually

Graber, D. (1994). Why voters fail information tests: Can the hurdles be overcome? Political
Communication, 11(4), 331–346. doi:10.1080/10584609.1994.9963044.
Graber, D. (1996). Say it with pictures. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and
Social Science, 546, 85–96. doi:10.1177/0002716296546001008.
Graber, D. (2001). Processing politics: Learning from television in the Internet age. Chicago, IL:
University of Chicago Press.
Gunter, B. (1987). Poor reception: Misunderstanding and forgetting broadcast news. New York,
NY: Routledge.
Hindman, D. (2009). Mass media flow and differential distribution of politically disputed
beliefs. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 86(4), 790–808. doi:10.1177/
107769900908600405.
Holden, C. (2004). The origin of speech. Science, 303(5662), 1316–1319.
Houston, S. D. (2004). The first writing script invention as history and process. New York, NY:
Cambridge University Press.
Hwang, Y., & Jeong, S. (2009). Revising the knowledge gap hypothesis: A meta-analysis of
thirty-five years of research. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 86(3),
513–532. doi:10.1177/107769900908600304.
Jennings, M. (1996). Political knowledge over time and across generations. Public Opinion
Quarterly, 60(2), 228–252. doi:10.1086/297749.
Jerit, J., Barabas, J., & Bolsen, T. (2006). Citizens, knowledge, and the information
environment. American Journal of Political Science, 50(2), 266–282. doi:10.1111/
j.1540-5907.2006.00183.x.
Karrasch, M., & Laine, M. (2003). Age, education and test performance on the Finnish
CERAD. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 108(2), 97–101. doi:10.1034/j.1600-0404.
2003.00037.x.
Katz, E., Adoni, H., & Parness, P. (1977). Remembering the news: What the picture adds to
recall. Journalism Quarterly, 54, 231–239. doi:10.1177/107769907705400201.
Keating, C., Randall, D., & Kendrick, T. (1999). Presidential physiognomies: Altered images,
altered perceptions. Political Psychology, 20(3), 593–610. doi:10.1111/0162-895X.00158.
Kim, S. (2008). Testing the knowledge gap hypothesis in South Korea. International Journal of
Public Opinion Research, 20(2), 193–210. doi:10.1093/ijpor/edn019.
Kwak, N. (1999). Revisiting the knowledge gap hypothesis education, motivation, and media
use. Communication Research,

Dokumen yang terkait

ANTARA IDEALISME DAN KENYATAAN: KEBIJAKAN PENDIDIKAN TIONGHOA PERANAKAN DI SURABAYA PADA MASA PENDUDUKAN JEPANG TAHUN 1942-1945 Between Idealism and Reality: Education Policy of Chinese in Surabaya in the Japanese Era at 1942-1945)

1 29 9

EVALUASI PENGELOLAAN LIMBAH PADAT MELALUI ANALISIS SWOT (Studi Pengelolaan Limbah Padat Di Kabupaten Jember) An Evaluation on Management of Solid Waste, Based on the Results of SWOT analysis ( A Study on the Management of Solid Waste at Jember Regency)

4 28 1

FAKTOR-FAKTOR YANG BERPENGARUH TERHADAP PENDAPATAN TENAGA KERJA PENGRAJIN ALUMUNIUM DI DESA SUCI KECAMATAN PANTI KABUPATEN JEMBER The factors that influence the alumunium artisans labor income in the suci village of panti subdistrict district jember

0 24 6

Implementasi Prinsip-Prinsip Good Corporate Governance pada PT. Mitra Tani Dua Tujuh (The Implementation of the Principles of Good Coporate Governance in Mitra Tani Dua Tujuh_

0 45 8

Improving the Eighth Year Students' Tense Achievement and Active Participation by Giving Positive Reinforcement at SMPN 1 Silo in the 2013/2014 Academic Year

7 202 3

Improving the VIII-B Students' listening comprehension ability through note taking and partial dictation techniques at SMPN 3 Jember in the 2006/2007 Academic Year -

0 63 87

Teaching speaking through the role play (an experiment study at the second grade of MTS al-Sa'adah Pd. Aren)

6 122 55

The Effectiveness of Computer-Assisted Language Learning in Teaching Past Tense to the Tenth Grade Students of SMAN 5 Tangerang Selatan

4 116 138

The correlation between listening skill and pronunciation accuracy : a case study in the firt year of smk vocation higt school pupita bangsa ciputat school year 2005-2006

9 128 37

Designing the Process Design Process 001

1 44 9