Knowledge of Word Theoretical Description

orders of groups are strikingly similar, 85 errors are developmental. This finding is also confirmed by Bailey, Madden, and Krashen 1974. They investigate 73 adults aged 17-55 years; classified as Spanish and non-Spanish-speaking members that separates in 8 ESL classes. From the above studies then it can be inferred that natural order of acquisition in ESL learners is irrespective of age Goldschneider and DeKeyser, 2001. On the other hand Ellis and Laporte 1997: 64 as quoted by Goldschneider and DeKeyser 2001 believe that the order acquisition can be explained by interaction between the characteristic of the elements to be acquired and general cognitive principles of inductive learning. The significance of this findings are intended to show that age of language learners does not refer to the natural order and sequence of their second language morpheme acquisition.

2. Knowledge of Word

a. Word Bloomfield as cited from Poole 1999: 10 considers that a word to be a minimum free form, a word, then, is a free form which does not consist entirely of two or more lesser free forms; in brief a word is a minimum free form. Carter 1998: 5 defines that a word is the minimum meaningful unit of language. On the other hand, Poole further argues that linguists devised the terms lexeme or lexical item to denote an item of vocabulary with a single referent. It can be inferred that a word has relation with its reference. Second language learners may be sufficient to show the equivalent meaning in their language. Word is familiar but eludes precise definition Taylor and Taylor, 1990 as quoted from Susilo 2001: 10. In some purposes linguists or dictionary treat words differently. Carroll et. all. 1971 as cited from Nation 1990 distinguish words entirely on the basis of form. Word form for some extent may become significant determination; for example: in the grammatical structure, word forms deal with units that are part of grammatical patterns. In that case, a word can be the subject of a sentence, the head of a modification structure, a structural signal in the form of a function word, etc. Lado 1984. These all are concern with form of word. Clearly, Laufer 1997 as quoted from Mukarto 1999: 31 proposes some aspects or features of word that learners need to attend to: 1. Word structure or morphology, i.e. the basic free morpheme and its derivational and, if any, inflectional morpheme. 2. Syntactic category, e.g. whether a word is a noun, an adjective or a verb; a verb in English may be mapped in to an adjective in Indonesia. 3. Relation with other words such as synonymy, hyponymy, antonymy, and common collocation and registers. b. The aspects are involved in knowing a word A language learner needs to understand that knowing a word is not merely able to show the equal meaning in his or her language. Richards 1976 via Read 2000: 25 has outlined eight assumptions which cover various aspects which are involved in knowing a word: 1. The vocabulary knowledge of native speakers continues to expand in adult life, in contrast to the relative stability of their grammatical competence. 2. Knowing a word means knowing the degree of probability of encountering that word in speech or print. For many words, we also know the sort of words most likely to be found associated with the word. 3. Knowing a word implies knowing the limitation on the use of word according to variations of function and situation. 4. Knowing a word means knowing the syntactic behavior associated with word. 5. Knowing a word entails knowledge of the underlying form of a word and the derivation that can be made from it. 6. Knowing a word entails knowledge of network of associations between that word and other words in the language. 7. Knowing a word means knowing the semantic value of a word. 8. Knowing a word means knowing many of the different meanings associated with a word. Richards, 1976: 83 The study confirms and modifies the fifth assumption. It deals with investigating Senior High School Students’ ability on the syntactic recognition of derivational suffixes of English words. Furthermore, this study also investigates the developmental patterns of syntactic recognition of English derivational suffixes. Meanwhile, Cronbach 1942 distinguishes five aspects of lexical knowledge: generalization knowing the definition, application knowledge about how to use, breadth of meaning knowing the different senses of word, precisions of meaning knowing how to use the word in different situations and availability knowing how to use the word productively as cited from Bogaards, 2000: 491. On the other hand, Cruse 1986 via Bogaards 2000: 492prefers to use the concept of lexical unit which is assumed to be the union of one stable meaning and a well-defined meaning. In accordance with this concept Bogaards proposes six aspects that may be learned about lexical unit in L2 as he assumes that L2 learners need to learn lexical units not only “words”: 1. form: Learners have to get acquainted with the written andor the spoken form of the unit. Knowing that a given form does indeed belonging to a given language seems to be a first stage of knowledge. 2. meaning: Knowledge of the semantic side of a lexical unit may come in different shape. One can have a vague notion, e.g. that haematin has something to do with the blood or that a beech is some kind of tree. 3. morphology: Lexical units have their own conditions on derivation and compounding. Gracefully and graciously have relationships to two different units which shared the form grace. Even when morphological mechanisms of the L2 are well understood, many of actual relationships have to be learned one by one see also Bogaards 1994: 53-7. Especially for productive it is difficult for L2 learners to know whether a given form is possible and in what sense it may be used. 4. syntax: A learner who knows the rule of syntax may make many mistakes by not applying the right rules to the lexical units. This applies especially to verbs and, to lesser degrees, to adjectives. Learners have to find out how many and what types of arguments are obligatory or possible with a verb in a given sense, or which prepositions have to be used with a verb or an adjective in some specific sense, e.g. with a particular lexical unit. 5. collocates: Whereas some lexical units, like very or red seem to be useable with a great number of other elements of a given category, others have a very restricted realm of use. Most of the collocations that seem so natural to native speakers make for great trouble for L2. 6. discourse: In what types of discourse and to what effect can lexical units like furthermore, moreover and what is more be used? Which lexical units are to be avoided when speaking to someone belonging to another ethnic group or when writing a letter of application? Knowledge of style, register and appropriateness of particular senses of a same grammar is notoriously difficult for L2 learners. Nation 1990 as quoted in Susilo 2001: 12 has stated that a word is to be learned or even acquired for receptive listening and reading and productive use speaking and writing. 1. Receptive Passive Knowledge Nation 1990 also argues that knowing a word entails being able to recognize it when it is heard what does it sound like? or when it is seen what does it look like?. This includes being able to distinguish it from words with similar form and being able to judge if the word sounds right or wrong. Receptive knowledge of a word reflects on having expectation of what grammatical pattern of the word will occur in. Knowing the verbs suggest involves the expectation that an object sometimes in the form of clause will follow the word. Knowing the noun music involves that it will not usually occur in the plural form. Furthermore, according to Nation 1990 knowing a word means knowing its form spoken and written, its position grammatical patterns, collocations, its function frequency, appropriateness, and its meaning concept and associations as cited in Laufer and Paribhakt 1998: 367. 2. Productive Knowledge Productive knowledge is the extension of receptive knowledge Nation, 1990. In this scope, knowing a word means knowing how to pronounce, how to write and spell it, how to use it in correct grammatical patterns along with the words it usually collocates with. Productive knowledge represents not using the word too often if it is a low-frequency word, and using it in suitable situation. Further, this knowledge also involves using the word to stand for meaning it represents and being able to think of suitable substitutes for the word if there are any. On the other hand, Lado 1984 as quoted in Susilo 2001: 14 mentions that an active vocabulary means that unit can be “recalled” almost instantaneously, put into sound through articulation of its phonemes, placed in its proper stress and intonation frame, into its proper structural positions and functions with its inflectional and derivational affixes in accord with the context. Most writers have assumed that passive vocabulary is larger than active Aitchison, 1989; Chanell, 1988; Laufer, 1998 as quoted from Laufer and Paribakht 1998: 369. They add that even tough no one has conclusively demonstrated how much larger it is or whether growth in passive vocabulary automatically growth in active vocabulary. c. Morphological processes of a word According to Quirk and Greenbaum 1973: 430, the chief processes of English word-formation by which the base may be modified are: 1. Affixation: a adding a prefix to the base, with or without a change of word-class b adding a suffix to the base, with or without a change of word-class 2. Conversion, i.e. assigning the base to a different word-class without changing its form zero affixation 3. Compounding, i.e. adding one base to another. They add that once a base has undergone a rule of word-formation, the derived word itself may become the base for another derivation. In line with those processes, Fromkin and Rodman 1996: 117 have stated that rules which relate to the formation of word and how morphemes combine to form new words called morphological rules. The significance of this theory is to show that the study relates to English word formation. It employs one process out of three processes as mentioned previously i.e. affixation. On the other hand, Radford, Atkinson, Britain, Clahsen, Spencer 1999: 166 explain lexeme as the more abstract term than term of ‘word’. They describe that cat and cats are the singular and plurals of one lexeme CAT; two ‘word forms’ of one lexeme. They claim that the singular and plural forms of a lexeme are the examples of inflections; CAT inflects for the plural by taking the suffix –s. On the other respect, they discuss the existence of the word read and reader in accordance with the lexemeword form distinction. They notice because of the addition -er to a verb creates a new lexeme and reader and read are distinct lexemes. Of course, each of them reader and read has a number of word forms: reader and readers in the case of READER, and reads, reading and read r εd in the case of READ. Moreover, the new lexeme is of a different syntactic category from that of original lexeme a verb become a noun. They claim that the creation of a lexeme is the province of derivational morphology or ‘derivation’. They also notice while adverbs ADV are often derived from adjectives by suffixation of –ly bad ~ bad-ly, noisy ~ noisi-ly , etc.. The other three categories N, A, and A can, however, readily be derived from each other. They add that preposition P does not participate in derivation in English or most other languages for that matter.

3. Lexical development