Methods Directory UMM :Data Elmu:jurnal:A:Applied Animal Behaviour Science:Vol68.Issue4.2000:

Table 1 Subject’s number, gender, birthdate and weaning date Subject number Gender Birthdate Weaning date 06F Male May 1, 1996 September 27, 1996 07F Female April 20, 1996 September 20, 1996 12F Female April 6, 1996 September 11, 1996 18F Female April 11, 1996 August 24, 1996 24F Female April 2, 1996 August 26, 1996 30F Male April 11, 1996 September 11, 1996 34F Male May 28, 1996 August 26, 1996 35F Male March 18, 1996 August 19, 1996 45F Male April 6, 1996 September 27, 1996 53F Female April 30, 1996 September 11, 1996 57F Male April 22, 1996 September 27, 1996 00F Female April 27, 1996 September 27, 1996 37F Male April 22, 1996 September 20, 1996 100F Female March 16, 1996 August 2, 1996 measured. The small paddock consisted of a mostly dirt substrate. Mares and foals were brought from the large field to the small paddock approximately five times a week until early July. After this time, they were brought to the small paddock one to two times a week. Mares and foals would spend several hours in the paddock each time they were housed there. Foals were weaned at approximately 4 months of age. Therefore, foals were not weaned at the same time, though they were sometimes weaned in groups of two or three. Throughout the weaning process, weaned foals remained in the pasture with unweaned foals and their mothers. The dams of weaned foals were moved to another pasture on the premises. The practice of weaning foals at 4 months of age is a standard practice at the Smithonia Breeding Center. Ž The caretakers distributed grain twice daily between 0830 and 0900 and between . 1700 and 1730 in buckets approximately 3 m apart. Throughout the study there were Ž . Ž . between 11 and 18 buckets from which 14 post-weaning to 28 pre-weaning animals fed. Distribution of food was not under the researcher’s control.

3. Methods

Observations were made on mares and foals from the middle of June until the last foal was weaned in September. Data were recorded for foals post-weaning until approximately 6 months of age. 3.1. Pre-weaning data collection Ž . Multiple 30-min random focal samples Altman, 1974 were conducted on each Ž . mare–foal pair weekly. All occurrence Altman, 1974 observations were also made during times when supplemental feed was given. Only agonistic data were recorded during supplemental feedings. Within each 30-min sample session the following types of data were recorded: agonistic encounters, non-aggressive encounters and spatial relation- ship. 3.1.1. Agonistic encounters: All agonistic encounters involving the focal mare and foal were recorded. For each encounter the initiator of the aggression and the recipient of the aggression were recorded. A description of the action of the aggressor and the reaction of the recipient were also recorded. The following behavioral patterns were scored as aggressive: 1. Head threat: The extension of the aggressor’s head and neck towards another individual while laying the ears against its head. 2. Bite threat: The aggressor’s ears were laid back. Its mouth was opened and a biting motion was made while it moved its head or whole body toward another animal, but no contact was made. 3. Bite: The aggressor’s ears were laid back and its teeth were closed on some body part of another animal. 4. Kick threat: Any of the three following behavioral sequences were scored as a threat to kick: a. The aggressor, with its ears laid back, made a rapid movement so as to place its hindquarters near another animal. b. The aggressor, with its ears laid back, raised one hind limb and held it in the air while in a position so as to potentially strike another. c. The aggressor, with its ears laid back, rapidly struck with one or both hind limbs towards another animal, but no contact was made. 5. Kick: With its ears laid back, one or both hindlimbs of the aggressor were projected outward rapidly and struck another animal. 6. Strike threat: The aggressor’s ears were laid back and its head and shoulders were oriented toward another individual. One or both forelimbs moved outward and forward toward the other animal but no contact was made. 7. Strike: The aggressor’s ears were laid back, and one or both forelimbs moved outward and struck the body of another animal. 8. Chase: With its ears laid back, the aggressor pursued an individual, for at least one full stride. The following behavioral patterns were scored as being submissive responses to one of the previously mentioned aggressive behaviors: 1. Head turn: The recipient of the aggression turned its head away from the aggressor. The recipient’s limbs did not move. 2. AÕoid: The recipient of the aggression moved away from the aggressor. 3.2. Non-aggressiÕe behaÕior The following behavioral pattern was recorded as a ‘‘win or lose’’ even though no aggression occurred: 1. Approach and retreat: This pattern was scored whenever one individual immedi- ately moved away from an animal that had just moved to within 2 m of it and had failed to exhibit an aggressive behavior. Only interactions in which there was a clear winner and loser were used to calculate the dominance hierarchy. In these instances, the animal that approached, threatened aggression, or aggressed was scored as a winner. The animal that responded either with a head turn, avoidance, or retreat was scored as a loser. Approach and aggressive behavior that did not elicit a submissive response was not used in the calculation of the dominance hierarchy. All aggressive behavior was used to calculate rates of aggression. An animal was considered subordinate to another animal if it lost five encounters and won none. If an animal won one or more encounters with a particular foal, then, in addition to the required five losses, that foal had to lose another two encounters for each win in order to be scored as the subordinate foal. If a dyad existed that did not meet the above-mentioned criteria, the foal that lost the greater number of encounters was scored subordinate. In order to determine the rank order among mares and foals, these dyadic relation- ships were evaluated, such that the highest ranking mare or foal submitted to the least number of mares or foals. The foal rank order was compared to the rank order of the Ž . dams, using Spearman’s rank order correlation coefficient rho SPSS to test for maternal influence on the rank of the foals. A correlation test was also performed on foal Ž . rank and foal birth order using Spearman’s rho. The wither height in inches and weight Ž . in grams of the foals were also subject to a Spearman’s rho correlation test. Body size Ž . measurements were taken 1 week prior to weaning approximately 4 months of age and Ž . during the last week of the study approximately 6 months of age . A Mann–Whitney test was used to test for any differences in rank among colts and fillies. Rates of aggression were calculated for individual mares and foals by dividing the focal animal’s total number of aggressive interactions by the total number of hours of observation in which agonistic behaviors were recorded. These aggression rates were broken down into rate of aggression given, rate of aggression received, rate of received aggression that was ignored and the rate of aggression given by the focal animal that was ignored by the recipient. All of the specific rates of aggression were calculated in the same manner as the overall rate of aggression. Aggression was also categorized as a percentage of aggression administered up the dominance hierarchy for each individual as well as the entire herd. The rates of aggression given, aggression received, and aggression ignored were all tested for correlation with the rank of the individual using a Spearman’s rho. Mares and foals were also ranked in accordance to their respective rates of giving and receiving aggression. These ranks were tested for correlation using the Spearman rank order correlation. Aggression rates per subordinate were calculated for mares and foals post-weaning. Total aggression to subordinates was divided by the total number of subordinates. This was then divided by the total number of hours of observation for each animal. An ANOVA was performed to test for any effects of gender on the type of aggression most often displayed. All correlations were performed one-tailed as the direction of the correlation was predicted prior to collection of data. 3.3. Spatial relationships The third category of data involved the focal animal’s spatial relationships with other animals. Every 2 min during each 30-min focal session the following data were recorded: 1. The identities of all animals within 1 m of the focal animal. 2. The identities of all animals greater than 1 m away but within 5 m of the focal animal. 3. The identities of all animals greater than 5 m away but within 10 m of the focal animal. These spatial relations were used to determine each mare and foal’s most preferred associate. The most preferred associate was defined as the animal that was observed within 5 m of the focal animal the greatest percentage of 2-min samples. A t-test for paired samples was used to test for an effect of the foal’s gender on the gender of its most preferred associate. Only fillies and colts were used for this test as the small number of early pre-pubertal castrates in the study group precluded their use in these analyses. In order to test whether or not a foal preferentially associated with the foal of its dam’s most preferred associate, the probability of the focal foal associating with the foal of the dam’s most preferred associate if association was random was determined. This was tested using a t-test for paired samples. Physical proximity rates were calculated for mares and foals by taking the total number of animals that were observed within 10 m of the focal animal and dividing by the total number of hours in which spatial data was recorded. These rates were then correlated with rank in mares and foals. A correlation test was also run between the sociability rates of mares and the sociability rates of their foals to test for any maternal influence on sociability. Aggression rates to preferred associates were calculated and a t-test for paired samples was used to test for any differences in aggression towards other foals and preferred associates.

4. Results

Dokumen yang terkait

PENGARUH BERBAGAI KONSENTRASI GARAM DALAM PEMBUATAN TELUR ASIN DARI BERBAGAI JENIS TELUR TERHADAP NILAI ORGANOLEPTIK SEBAGAI SUMBER BELAJAR - UMM Institutional Repository

1 2 15

ANALISIS KANDUNGAN GIZI CACING HONINGKA (Siphonosoma australe-australe) YANG BERPOTENSI SEBAGAI SUMBER PANGAN MASYARAKAT PESISIR SOMBU KEPULAUAN WAKATOBI - UMM Institutional Repository

1 9 21

EFEKTIVITAS EKSTRAK DAUN ASAM JAWA (Tamarindus indica L.) TERHADAP DAYA HAMBAT Staphylococcus epidermidis SEBAGAI SUMBER BELAJAR BIOLOGI - UMM Institutional Repository

0 0 22

BAB 1 PENDAHULUAN 1.1 Latar Belakang - EFEKTIVITAS EKSTRAK DAUN ASAM JAWA (Tamarindus indica L.) TERHADAP DAYA HAMBAT Staphylococcus epidermidis SEBAGAI SUMBER BELAJAR BIOLOGI - UMM Institutional Repository

0 0 11

BAB II TINJAUAN PUSTAKA 2.1 Tinjauan Tentang Asam Jawa (Tamarindus indica) - EFEKTIVITAS EKSTRAK DAUN ASAM JAWA (Tamarindus indica L.) TERHADAP DAYA HAMBAT Staphylococcus epidermidis SEBAGAI SUMBER BELAJAR BIOLOGI - UMM Institutional Repository

0 0 19

BAB III METODOLOGI PENELITIAN 3.1 Jenis Penelitian - EFEKTIVITAS EKSTRAK DAUN ASAM JAWA (Tamarindus indica L.) TERHADAP DAYA HAMBAT Staphylococcus epidermidis SEBAGAI SUMBER BELAJAR BIOLOGI - UMM Institutional Repository

2 8 26

Knowledge, Attitude and Behaviour Related to Transmission and Prevention of Malaria in Mentawai Island District, West Sumatera Province

0 0 8

PERILAKU MASYARAKAT DALAM PENGELOLAAN SANITASI LINGKUNGAN PEMUKIMAN DI PERKEBUNAN KOPI KABUPATEN JEMBER (The Behaviour Of Society In The Management Of Environmental Sanitation At Coffee Plantation Residence In Jember Regency)

0 0 9

PERILAKU PENCARIAN PENGOBATAN TERHADAP PENYAKIT TIDAK MENULAR DI DESA SULAHO, KECAMATAN LASUSUA, KABUPATEN KOLAKA UTARA (Health Seeking Behaviour of Non Communicable Disease in Sulaho Village, Lasusua Sub District, North Kolaka Regency)

0 0 10

HEALTH SEEKING BEHAVIOUR PADA PERSALINAN SUKU KAILI DA’A DI DESA WULAI KABUPATEN MAMUJU UTARA SULAWESI BARAT (Health Seeking Behaviour in Delivery Process Among Kaili Da’a Ethnic Groups In Mamuju Utara, West Celebes)

0 0 8