THE MIND LEARNING TO READ

THE MIND LEARNING
TO READ
Roelien Herholdt & Prof. Elbie Henning
2015

CONTENTS
  A baseline
  A neuroscience perspective
  Contextualisation
  Preparation for later learning to read
  Speech circuits
  Visual circuits

  Learning to read
  Implications for education

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This presentation draws widely on the works of:
  Prof Stanislas Dehaene – Reading in the brain
  Dr Jenny Thomson – University of London
  Dr Duncan Milne –Teaching the brain to read

  Prof Leonard White and Prof Dale Purvis – Duke University
  All other sources can be found under references

SOME QUESTIONS
True or false
  The foundations for reading are laid when children start with grade R or
in some cases the last years of nursery school
  Areas facilitating reading is only found in the left hemisphere of the
brain
  Whole language approaches or balanced language approaches are the
optimal way to teach reading

WHY NEUROSCIENCE?
  Understanding a system, in this case the brain, can assist in
understanding how to this system works
  Empowering teachers and people working with children with an
understanding of the neuroscience of reading can lead to better
ways of assisting children
  An understanding of the underlying science can assist in the
development and testing of teaching methodologies and materials


CONTEXTUALISATION
Literacy, including reading, must be understood within the South African
context
South Africa is a multilingual country
o  Bi-/multilingualism is the norm rather than the exception
o  Languages differ in terms of their regularity – how well sounds/
phonemes map onto letters
o  Which languages you put together is important

TYPES OF LANGUAGES
  Logographic languages
  Transparent languages
o  Letter-sound (grapheme-phoneme) connections are regular
o  Phonological awareness – predictor of reading achievement
o  Phoneme most important component
  Less transparent languages
o  Lots of irregularities or exceptions
o  Onset and rime patterns become more important


LEARNING TO READ
Children start on the path to becoming readers in the first year of life
o  Visual development – invariant visual recognition
o  Linguistic development – speech comprehension






P R E PA R AT I O N F O R R E A D I N G
Prenatally and first six months after birth
 Rhythm of native language in utero
 Linguistic contrasts e.g. /ba/ /ga/
 Left superior temporal region – analysis of speech sounds
 Temporal lobe – extracting phonemes, words and sentences
 Left inferior prefrontal region - Broca’s area – previously
thought to only involve speech production and grammatical
skills, activated in babies listening to speech
 Predisposition for acquiring a language

 Prosody – myth of left hemisphere dominance


P R E PA R AT I O N F O R R E A D I N G
First three years –tuning to native language
 six months – vowels of native language
 one year – consonants of native language
o  Japanese babies /r/ /l/ and in South Africa?
o  Discards speech combinations not in native language
o  Speech segments occurring most often become first
words
 two to three years
o  vocabulary increases by 10 – 20 words per day
o  Basic grammatical rules of language

READY TO READ
  Age five to six
  Vocabulary of several 1000 words in native language
  Basic grammatical rules of language
  Visual system developed invariant recognition

o  Maximal plasticity or a sensitive period
  Sophisticated speech circuits, which will assist in making sense of the
written word

VISUAL CIRCUITS
  Simultaneously to the speech circuits the visual circuits develop
  Infants learns to
o  parse visual scenes into objects and to track them, even when
they are concealed for a period of time
o  recognise faces – by 9 months they specialise in recognition of
human faces

VISUAL CIRCUITS
  One year olds
o  can discriminate between objects using contours, texture, and
whether they are convex or concave
o  when viewing an object from several view points they can infer
its three dimensional shape, using the type of edge junctions
(T, Y or L)
  Two year olds can break an object down into its parts or elements

  Five to six year olds have developed invariant visual recognition

STAGES IN READING
Logographic or pictorial stage
  Recognises words as objects
  Uses color, shape, letter orientation and curvature
  Exploits superficial cues
  Very artificial form of reading
  The right occipito-temporal region distinguishes consonant strings
from words – bilateral processing
  Stage brief in transparent languages

PHONOLOGICAL STAGE
  Grapheme-phoneme links as well as link between spoken and written language
  Phonemic awareness – spoken words consist of phonemes
  Explicit teaching – alphabetic principle: phonemes map onto graphemes
  Word length and grapheme complexity increase reading time
  Illiterates can discriminate sounds, detect rhyme, etc. but struggle with
substitution
•  Mastery of alphabetic principle changes brain wiring

•  Visual system breaks words into graphemes
•  Parts of speech systems adapt to explicit representation of phonemes

PHONEMES OR GRAPHEMES
FIRST?
  Spiral causality
o  Grapheme awareness focus attention on phonemes
o  Phonemic awareness enhances grapheme awareness

  Phonological stage is characterised by
o  regularisation mistakes of irregular words such as “said” will be
read as “sa-it”, “key” will be read as “kay”
o  Diffculty reading words with complex consonant structures, e.g.
CCCVCC such as in “strict”

ORTHOGRAPHIC STAGE
  Reading time no longer determined by word length or grapheme complexity
  Higher frequency words read faster than rare words
  Reading becomes more fluent
  Parallelism as opposed to serial processing

•  Up to 8 letters at a time
•  Still processes every letter though
  Efficiency increases

THE LETTERBOX
  Also called the visual word form area
  Located in the left lateral occipito-temporal sulcus (valley), next to
the fusiform gyrus (hill)
  This area is activated, irrespective of the language which is read,
the reading direction (left to right or right to left) or the type of
language

T WO R E A D I N G PAT H WAY S
Phonological decoding route
  Depends on phoneme-grapheme correspondence
  Generative – “self-teaching effect”
  Steps:
o  Segmentation
o  Transcoding – link grapheme to phoneme
o  Fusion or concatenation

  Assess through pseudo-words, e.g. labbit
o  Lexicalisation, e.g. labbit is read as rabbit
o  Additions, omissions, inversions and substitution

T WO R E A D I N G PAT H WAY S
Direct access or lexical route
  After lots of repetition
o  Develops only after years of practice
o  Creates illusion of whole word reading though fast and efficient automatisation
of processes
  Depends on establishment of a direct connection between visual and auditory systems
  Leads to less mistakes and is faster
  Used most often by fluent readers
o  Left hemispheric dominance for processing in reading occurs
o  Prosody still processed in right hemisphere
  Assess using irregular words, e.g. said
o  Mistake = regularisation e.g. sa-it





H OW T H I S L O O K S I N T H E
BRAIN?

SOME UNDISPUTED FACTS
  Reading changes the brain
o  Cortical areas for face, object and colour recognition become attuned to
graphemes and written word

  Reading improves reading
o  Left inferior prefrontal cortex

  Poor readers’ reading achievement gets progressively worse without
intervention
  Reading must be taught explicitly
o  Children do not acquire reading spontaneously
o  Learning takes time to master – in more opaque languages learning to read takes
longer

MIRROR READING/WRITING

o  Called boustrophedon
o  Natural process where visual invariance is applied to graphemes and words

DYSLEXIA
  Neurologically based – phonological pathway
  Often hereditary
  Leads to problems with reading, writing and spelling
  Associated with difficulties in
o  Concentration
o  Short term memory
o  Organisation


HELPFUL STRATEGIES
  Explicit teaching of phonemic awareness
  Explicit teaching of alphabetical principle
  Phonics programme must be structured and sequential, e.g. teach regular
frequently used phonemes first
  Simultaneous teaching of graphemes and phonemes
  Multisensory – feel pronunciation, use concrete letters, hear & say
  Metacognitive, e.g. LCWC for irregular words
  Reduce memory and attention load

BRUCE MCCANDLISS

BRUCE MCCANDLISS
  Phonics vs whole language experiment
  WL did better on first 30 words learned, but on learning the second 30
words they started forgetting the first words
  Phonics group took longer to master the grapheme-phoneme combinations,
but:
o  Improved steadily
o  Did better on encountering new words
o  Remembered previously learned words better, even with no revision

TEACHING
  Should we aim to increase verbal vocabulary?
  Should we teach letter sounds, letter names or both?
  Should our teaching of letter formation be linked to our teaching of
phonemes, spelling and reading?
  Should we teach graphemes or phonemes first?
  Whole language, phonics approach or balanced language approach?

REFERENCES
REFERENCES
  Bhatt, R.S., Hayden, A., Bertin, E. & Joseph, J. (2006). Infants’ perception of
information along object bounderies: Concavities versus convexities. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 94(2), 91-113.
  Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and representations. Oxford: Basil Blackwell
  De Haan, M., Johnson, M.H. & Halit, H. (2003). Development of face sensitive event
related potentials during infancy: a review. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 51(1),
45-58.
  Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In Patterson, K. E.,
Marshall, J. C. & Colheart, M. (Eds.), Surface dyslexia: Cognitive and
neuropsychological studies of phonological reading. Hilldale: Erlbuam. Pp 301-330

REFERENCES
REFERENCES
  Gathers, A.D., Bhatt, R., Corbly, C.R., Farley, A.B. &Joseph, J.E. (2004). Developmental shifts
in cortical loci for face and object recognition. NeuroReport, 15(10), 1549-1553.
  Kellman.P. J., & Spelke, E.S. (1983). Perception of partly occluded objects in infancy. Cognitive
Psychology, 15, 483 – 524
  Kraebel, K.S. , West, R.N. & Gerhardstein, P. (2007). The influence of training views on
infants’ long-term memory for simple 3D shapes. Developmental Psychobiology, 49(4), 406-420.
  Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Early language acquisition: Cracking the speecg code. Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 5(11), 831-843.
  Mehler, J., Jusczyk, P., Halsted, N., Bertoncini, J. & Amiel-Tison, C. (1988). A precursor of
language acquisition in young infants. Cognition, 29, 143-178

REFERENCES
REFERENCES
  Morais, J., Bertelson, P., Cary, L. & Alegria, J. (1986). Literacy training and speech
segmentation. Cognition, 24, 45-64.
  Pascalis, O., De Haan, M. & Nelson, V.A. (2002). Is face processing species-specific during
the first year of life? Science, 296(5571), 1321-1323
  Pena, M., Maki, A., Kovacic, D., Dehaene-Lambertz, G. Koizumi, H., Bouquet, F. & Mehler
(2003). Sounds of silence: An optical topography study of language recognition at birth.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(20), 11702 -11705
  Robinson, A.J. & Pascalis, O. (2004). Development of flexible visual recognition menory in
human infants. Developmental Science, 7(5), 527-533.

REFERENCES
  Share, D.L. (1999). Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of
the self teaching hypothesis. Journal of experimental Child Psychology, 72(2), 95-129.
  Shuwairi, S.M., Albert, M.K., Johnson, S.P. (2007). Discrimination of possible and
impossible objects in infancy. Psychological Science, 18(4), 303-307.
  Son, J.Y., Smith, L.B. & Goldstone, R.L. (2008). Simplicity and generalisation: Shortcutting abstraction in children’s object categorisations. Cognition, 108(3), 626-638.
  Wang, S.H. & Baillargeon, R. (2008). Detecting impossible changes in infancy: a threesystem account. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(1), 17-23.

REFERENCES
  Zocolotti, P., De Luca, M., Di Pace, E., Gasperini, F., Judica, A. &
Spinelli, D. (2005). Word length effect in early reading and in
developmental dyslexia. Brain and Lamguage, 93(3), 369-373.