getting work for the nonet non-given beneficiary more probable b. getting the nonet work get men for the navy non-given beneficiary get the navy men less probable gets them given beneficiary a decent wage gets a decent wage for them less probable get you

60 presented in Table 4.3 See also Appendix 3. The examples below illustrate the probability of the occurrence of instances possessing non-given and given beneficiary. 1

a. getting work for the nonet non-given beneficiary more probable b. getting the nonet work

less probable 2

a. get men for the navy non-given beneficiary

more probable

b. get the navy men less probable

3

a. gets them given beneficiary a decent wage

more probable

b. gets a decent wage for them less probable

4

a. get you given beneficiary some drops

more probable

b. get some drops for you less probable

This result of the effect of givenness of beneficiary confirms the finding of the earlier studies on alternations. As non-given beneficiary favors benefactive PP construction, it simply agrees the claim that given information precedes the non- given one in alternations Collins 1995; Bresnan et al. 2007; Theijssen et al. 2009. It puts the non-given information at the very end of the sentence, whereas the given information comes just after verb postverbal position. Whereas beneficiary and theme should be in complement Bresnan 2007, the result suggests that the givenness of theme does not significantly affect the choice of benefactive construction. The p value of the feature givenness of theme is 0.874 far from being significant. Its 95 confidence interval CI is too way spread. It has lower limit less than 1 and the upper limit of 5.900. The coefficient is positive of 0.133 which cannot be said to be in balance with the 61 givenness of beneficiary See Table 4.3 and Appendix 3. Ideally, when the givenness of beneficiary possesses positive coefficient, the givenness of theme should have negative coefficient. This way, the idea of given information preceding non-given will stay correct. However, this phenomenon is explainable since the feature of givenness of theme is insignificant to the choice of benefactive construction. Partially, the feature givenness of beneficiary has stronger effects on the choice of benefactive construction. When the feature of givenness of beneficiary is tested in one on one binary logistic model, the classification table of the feature givenness of beneficiary toward the choice of benefactive alternation shows 83.8 of the model accuracy. Ignoring the other features, the feature givenness of beneficiary and the intercept are able to predict the occurrence of the benefactive PP or double object construction with 83.8 accuracy. In addition, the omnibus and model summary tables support the idea by giving the evidence that the single feature of givenness of beneficiary reduce the -2 log likehood of 246.260. The model with intercept only owns -2 log likehood of 535.883. while after adding the variable of givenness of beneficiary the -2 log likehood decrease to 289.623. Applying the variable of givenness of beneficiary in the model will be able to explain the choice of benefactive construction as Nagelkerke R square suggests of 0.623. The model fit accuracy, -2 log likehood, and Nagelkerke R square are presented in Appendix 3. The coefficient and the odds ratio of the feature givenness of beneficiary increase considerably to 5.010 and 149.860 from the ones when it was applied with the other thirteen features See the Table 4.5 below. 62 Variables in the Equation of beneficiary givenness B S.E. Wald df Sig. ExpB 95 C.I.for EXPB Lower Upper Step 1 a beneficiarygivenness 5.010 .601 69.429 1 .000 149.860 46.123 486.919 Constant -5.920 .655 81.614 1 .000 .003 a. Variables entered on step 1: beneficiarygivenness. Table 4.5 Variable givenness of beneficiary removed from full model

4.1.2 Animacy of Beneficiary

The second significant feature animacy of beneficiary was coded to categorical variable in the logistic model beneficiary=animate is 1, and beneficiary=inanimate is 2 See Appendices 1 and 2 for the detailed identification and annotating process of the benefactive data. The default of the feature is inanimate, whereas the default of the benefactive construction is benefactive PP. The p value of the feature animacy of beneficiary reaches the number 0.035 See Table 4.3 and Appendix 3 which is less than the conventional significance level 0.05. Thus, the feature animacy of beneficiary is proven to be relevant to the choice of benefactive construction. The crosstabulation table of animacy of beneficiary toward ditransitivity shows that 98 of inanimate beneficiary take benefactive PP construction, only the rest 2 of inanimate beneficiary take double object construction. Conversely, 51.8 of animate beneficiary take double object construction, and the rest 48.2 take benefactive PP construction. Both statistics say that animate argument comes before inanimate one. 63 Animacy of Beneficiary toward Ditransitivity Crosstabulation Ditransitivity Total benefactive construction prepositional construction Animacy of Beneficiary animate beneficiary Count 155 144 299 within Animacy of Beneficiary 51.8 48.2 100.0 within Ditransitivity 98.7 59.3 74.8 of Total 38.8 36.0 74.8 inanimate beneficiary Count 2 99 101 within Animacy of Beneficiary 2.0 98.0 100.0 within Ditransitivity 1.3 40.7 25.3 of Total .5 24.8 25.3 Total Count 157 243 400 within Animacy of Beneficiary 39.3 60.8 100.0 within Ditransitivity 100.0 100.0 100.0 of Total 39.3 60.8 100.0 Table 4.6 Crosstabulation of animacy of beneficiary toward ditransitivity In addition, the result shows that the feature animacy of beneficiary possesses positive coefficient B of 2.268. It means that the default inanimate beneficiary favors the default benefactive PP construction. The size of the effect of the feature is explainable through the odds ratio expB of 9.657. It suggests that inanimate beneficiary is more than 9 times likely to take benefactive PP construction rather than animate beneficiary. The 95 confidence interval CI supports the claim, showing that inanimate beneficiary tends to choose benefactive PP construction between 1.175 to 79.383 times. Conversely, if the beneficiary is animate, the sentence will tend to appear in the double object construction. The coefficient B, odds ratio expB, and 95 CI above are presented in Table 4.3 See also Appendix 3. The examples below illustrate the 64 probability of the occurrence of instances possessing inanimate and animate beneficiary. 4

a. getting water for their shipping inanimate

more probable

b. getting their shipping water less probable

5

a. getting funding for clinical trials inanimate

more probable

b. get clinical trials funding less probable

6

a. get thee animate a wife

more probable

b. gets a wife for thee less probable

7

a. get you animate anything

more probable

b. get anything for you less probable

This result of the effect of animacy of beneficiary confirms the finding of the earlier studies on alternations. As inanimate beneficiary favors benefactive PP construction, it simply agrees the claim that animate argument precedes inanimate one in alternations Bock et al 1992; Thompson 1995; Bresnan and Hay 2008; Lamers et al. 2008; Theijssen et al. 2009. The benefactive PP puts inanimate argument, which is the beneficiary, after the postverbal NP position in the end of the construction. Beneficiary and theme should be in complement Bresnan 2007 and the result suggests the same way. Although animacy of theme does not significantly affect the choice of benefactive construction, its relation with animacy of beneficiary is in harmony. The coefficient of animacy of theme is negative of - 1.055 which implies that animate theme favors benefactive PP construction. On the other hand, the coefficient of animacy of beneficiary is as we know positive of 65 2.268, thus inanimate beneficiary favors benefactive PP. This way, the statistic show balance in the way animate theme choosing benefactive PP and inanimate beneficiary choosing benefactive PP construction. This combination left the fact that the animate argument precedes the inanimate one. Partially, the feature animacy of beneficiary also has strong effect although doubtfully accurate toward the choice of benefactive construction. When the feature of animacy of beneficiary is tested in one on one binary logistic model, the classification table of the feature beneficiary of beneficiary toward the choice of benefactive alternation shows 63.5 of the model accuracy. Ignoring the other features, the feature animacy of beneficiary and the intercept are able to predict the occurrence of the benefactive PP or double object construction with 63.5 accuracy. In addition, the omnibus and model summary tables support the idea by giving the evidence that the single feature of animacy of beneficiary reduce the -2 log likehood of 102.137. The model with intercept only owns -2 log likehood of 535.883, while after adding the variable of animacy of beneficiary the -2 log likehood decrease to 433.745. Applying the variable of animacy of beneficiary in the model will only be able to explain the choice of benefactive construction as Nagelkerke R square suggests of 0.305. The model fit accuracy, -2 log likehood, and Nagelkerke R square are presented in Appendix 3. The coefficient and the odds ratio of the feature givenness of beneficiary increase significantly to 3.976 and 53.281 from the ones when it was applied with the other thirteen features See Table 4.7 below. 66 Variables in the Equation of Beneficiary Animacy B S.E. Wald df Sig. ExpB 95 C.I.for EXPB Lower Upper Step 1 a beneficiaryanimacy 3.976 .724 30.192 1 .000 53.281 12.903 220.011 Constant -4.049 .751 29.087 1 .000 .017 a. Variables entered on step 1: beneficiaryanimacy. Table 4.7 Variable animacy of beneficiary removed from full model

4.1.3 Pronominality of Theme

The next significant feature pronominality of theme was coded to categorical variable in the logistic model theme=pronoun is 1, and theme=non- pronoun is 2 See Appendices 1 and 2 for the detailed identification and annotating process of the benefactive data. The default of the feature is non- pronoun, whereas the default of the benefactive construction is benefactive PP. The p value of the feature pronominality of theme reaches the number 0.010 See Table 4.3 and Appendix 3 which is less than the conventional significance level 0.05. Thus, the feature pronominality of theme is proven to be relevant to the choice of benefactive construction. The crosstabulation table of pronominality of theme toward ditransitivity shows that 91.7 of pronoun theme take benefactive PP construction, only the rest 8.3 of pronoun theme take double object construction. However, benefactive construction is not sensitive toward non-pronoun theme. The result shows that 57.7 of non-pronoun themes take benefactive PP construction, and 67 the rest 42.3 take double object construction. The statistic of pronoun theme strongly suggests that as possible pronoun appears in postverbal position. Pronominality of Theme toward Ditransitivity Crosstabulation Ditransitivity Total benefactive construction prepositional construction Pronominality of Theme pronoun theme Count 3 33 36 within Pronominality of Theme 8.3 91.7 100.0 within Ditransitivity 1.9 13.6 9.0 of Total .8 8.3 9.0 non- pronoun theme Count 154 210 364 within Pronominality of Theme 42.3 57.7 100.0 within Ditransitivity 98.1 86.4 91.0 of Total 38.5 52.5 91.0 Total Count 157 243 400 within Pronominality of Theme 39.3 60.8 100.0 within Ditransitivity 100.0 100.0 100.0 of Total 39.3 60.8 100.0 Table 4.8 Crosstabulation of pronominality of theme toward ditransitivity The table of variables in the equation shows that the feature pronominality of theme possesses negative coefficient B of -2.183. It means that the non- default pronoun theme favors the default benefactive PP construction. The size of the effect of the feature is explainable through the odds ratio expB of 0.113. It suggests that pronoun theme is 0.113 times to take non-default double object construction or around 9 times likely to take benefactive PP construction. Given 100 occurrences of pronoun theme, 11 will likely take double object construction, while 89 instances favor benefactive PP construction. The 95 confidence interval CI supports the claim, showing that pronoun theme tends to choose 68 double object construction between 0.022 to 0.588 times. It means that pronoun theme tends to take benefactive PP construction between almost 2 times and more than 45 times. In line with this fact, if the theme is non-pronoun, the sentence tendency to appear in the double object construction is bigger than if the theme is pronoun. The coefficient B, odds ratio expB, and 95 CI above are presented in Table 4.3 See also Appendix 3. The examples below illustrate the probability of the occurrence of instances possessing non-pronoun and pronoun theme. 9

a. play them pronoun theme for us

more probable

b. play us them unacceptable

10

a. fix it pronoun theme for you

more probable

b. fix you it unacceptable

11

a. get him a shirt non-pronoun theme

more probable

b. get him it unacceptable

12

a. fixing herself some food non-pronoun theme

more probable

b. fixing herself them unacceptable

While the previous studies suggest that in double object construction, pronoun precedes a lexical NP are far more frequent than those in which two lexical NPs occur Thompson 1990; Collins 1995; Bresnan et al 2007, this study suggests that when 2 pronouns occur, the theme will come first. As pronoun theme favors benefactive PP construction, the beneficiary will be positioned after the theme. It means that even when the beneficiary is a pronoun, it should come after the pronoun theme which comes postverbal. 69 Beneficiary and theme should be in complement Bresnan et al. 2007 and the result suggests the same way. Although pronominality of beneficiary does not significantly affect the choice of benefactive construction, its relation with pronominality of theme is in harmony. The coefficient of pronominality of beneficiary is positive of 1.577 which proposes that non-pronoun beneficiary favors benefactive PP construction. On the other hand, the coefficient of pronominality of theme is as we know negative of -2.183. This way, the statistic shows balance where non-pronoun beneficiary chooses benefactive PP and animate theme chooses benefactive PP construction. In benefactive PP construction with pronoun theme and non-pronoun beneficiary, pronoun precedes non-pronoun. Partially, the feature pronominality of theme has relatively weak effect and doubtfully accurate toward the choice of benefactive construction. When the feature of pronominality of theme is tested in one on one binary logistic model, the classification table of the feature pronominality of theme toward the choice of benefactive alternation shows 60.8 of the model accuracy. Ignoring the other features, the feature pronominality of theme along with the intercept are able to predict the occurrence of the benefactive PP or double object construction with 63.5 accuracy. In addition, the omnibus and model summary tables suggest that the single feature pronominality of theme is quite weak to determine the choice of benefactive alternation. The evidence shows that the single feature of pronominality of theme only reduces the -2 log likehood of 19.269. The model with intercept has -2 log likehood of 535.883, while after adding the variable of pronominality of theme the -2 log likehood decreases only 19.269 to 516.614. 70 Applying the variable of pronominality of theme in the model will only be able to explain the choice of benefactive construction as Nagelkerke R square suggests of 0.064. The very small number of Nagelkerke R square tell us that when appears alone, pronominality of theme is barely able to explain the choice of benefactive construction. The model fit accuracy, -2 log likehood, and Nagelkerke R square are presented in Appendix 3. The coefficient and the odds ratio of the feature pronominality of theme is stable at around -2 and 0.1 to -2.088 and 0.124 from the ones when it was applied with the other thirteen features. Variables in the Equation of Theme Pronominality B S.E. Wald df Sig. ExpB 95 C.I.for EXPB Lower Upper Step 1 a themepronominality -2.088 .612 11.626 1 .001 .124 .037 .412 Constant 4.486 1.211 13.727 1 .000 88.733 a. Variables entered on step 1: themepronominality. Table 4.9 Variable pronominality of theme removed from the full model

4.1.4 Definiteness of Theme

The next significant feature definiteness of theme was coded to categorical variable in the logistic model theme=definite is 1, and theme=indefinite is 2 See Appendices 1 and 2 for the detailed identification and annotating process of the benefactive data. The default of the feature is indefinite, whereas the default of the benefactive construction is benefactive PP. The p value of the feature definiteness of theme reaches the number 0.04 See Table 4.3 and Appendix 3 which is less than the conventional significance level 0.05. Thus, the feature 71 definiteness of theme is proven to be relevant to the choice of benefactive construction. The crosstabulation table of definiteness of theme toward ditransitivity shows that 87.7 of definite theme take benefactive PP construction, only the rest 12.5 of definite theme take double object construction. On the other hand, the effect of indefinite theme seems to be a little bit weaker toward benefactive construction. The result shows that 56.3 of indefinite themes take double object construction construction, and the rest 43.7 take benefactive PP. The statistic of definiteness of theme suggests that as possible definite argument appears in before indefinite one. Definiteness of Theme toward Ditransitivity Crosstabulation Ditransitivity Total benefactive construction prepositional construction Definiteness of Theme definite theme Count 19 136 155 within Definiteness of Theme 12.3 87.7 100.0 within Ditransitivity 12.1 56.0 38.8 of Total 4.8 34.0 38.8 indefinite theme Count 138 107 245 within Definiteness of Theme 56.3 43.7 100.0 within Ditransitivity 87.9 44.0 61.3 of Total 34.5 26.8 61.3 Total Count 157 243 400 within Definiteness of Theme 39.3 60.8 100.0 within Ditransitivity 100.0 100.0 100.0 of Total 39.3 60.8 100.0 Table 4.10 Crosstabulation of definiteness of theme toward ditransitivity 72 The result in the table of variables in the equation shows that the feature pronominality of theme possesses negative coefficient B of -1.340. It means that the non-default definite theme favors the default benefactive PP construction. The size of the effect of the feature is explainable through the odds ratio expB of 0.262. It suggests that definite theme is 0.262 times to take non-default double object construction or around 4 times likely to take benefactive PP construction. Given 100 occurrences of definite theme, 26 will likely take double object construction, while 74 instances favor benefactive PP construction. The 95 confidence interval CI supports the claim, showing that definite theme tends to choose double object construction between 0.105 to 0.656 times. It means that definite theme tends to take benefactive PP construction between almost 2 times and more than 9 times. Equally, if the theme is indefinite, the sentence tendency to appear in the double object construction is bigger than if the theme is definite. The coefficient B, odds ratio expB, and 95 CI above are presented in Table 4.3 See also Appendix 3. The examples below illustrate the probability of the occurrence of instances possessing indefinite and definite theme. 13

a. fixed the dinner definite theme for her family

more probable

b. fixed her family the dinner less probable

14

a. make the frames definite for Dodge Ram pickup more probable b. make Dodge Ram pickup the frames

less probable 15

a. get me a stove indefinite theme