Adverbs as Nominals ‘Uncle’,

nominalized counterpart with basically the same meaning wdkuftjuD: tuik a-kri: building + Nom + big ‘big building’ making the construction a compound noun N + N. As noted earlier, the same general meaning can be framed with aom sau: - juD:aomwdkuf kri: sau: tuik big + NomAtr + building ‘big building’. It appears at first glance that the head-final typology is violated in this construction, and while there are three ways to formulate the attributive relation, the regular N + V construction is, in fact, a reduced clause pragmatically conventionalized into a whole concept. Without the clause-final marker onf sany, or one of its variants, the attributive relation is not highly predicational. 10 Stative verbs are not strongly predicational or assertive even when they are the main verb in a sentence. The three constructions form a cline of attribution with varying degrees of nominality or predictability, as shown in table 20. Nominal ³ ´ Verbal Predicable N t- a- + V [stative] N V [stative] V [stative] + aom sau N V [stative] + onf. sany. N N V [stative] + onf sany or variants wdkuftjuD : tuik a-kri: building + big ‘big building’ wdkufjuD : tuik kri: building + big ‘main building’ juD:aomwdkuf kri: sau tuik big + Nom + building ‘big building’ quality of the building juD:onf.wdkuf kri: sany. tuik big + Nom + building ‘building which is big’ predication of the building wdkufjuD:onf tuik kri: sany building + big + Nom ‘the building is big’ statement about the building Table 20. The spectrum of nominality of attributive constructions

3.5.3 Adverbs as Nominals

Adverbs typically demonstrate a mixed categoriality unlike other lexical classes and universally tend to be derived from other noun, verb, or adjective words Givón 1984:77. The derivational path often determines their semantic and morphological characteristics. Such is the case with Burmese “adverbs.” What has been characterized in other grammars of Burmese as adverbs are analyzable as double-verb modifiers to the matrix head verb or as derived nominals in a nominal compound, depending on the derivational process used in the construction. Adverbial concepts are normally formed from a base verb, which is reduplicated e.g., \ref\ref mran mran fast + fast = ‘quickly’ and situated in the modifier position immediately preceding the matrix verb, or from a verb stem that is nominalized with the t- a- nominalizing prefix e.g., t\ref a-mran Nom + fast = ‘quickly’. Another variant, though 10 At first glance, to say the sentence final nominalizer on f sany marks a construction as more predicational seems like a contradiction of function. Yet, by the presence of on f sany the construction is elongated not iconically reduced and therefore blending of the concepts into one whole concept is less likely. Also, the presence of on f sany indicates a boundary of a unit of perception, which though that unit is a nominal, when it separates the stative verb unit from the head N, then it does not immediately bind to the head nominal but separates it and invokes a sense of predicating the verb of the nominal unit. less common, is with the nominalizer prefix w- ta- e.g., w\ref ta- mran ‘quickly’ see 4.2. The overt nominalization process operating in adverbials must be accounted for. The current analysis acknowledges the extensive process of nominalization and the way in which this process is consistently reflected in the wider patterns in the language as a whole and does not need to posit a separate form class for adverbials. It is the construction and the semantic profile that contributes the sense of manner, which is the basic semantic function for what have been classified as lexical adverbs. Adverbials are shaped on the structure of a basic clause 66a by the addition of the features just described and exemplified in 66b–e. 66 a. unmodified action clause armifvS0if: onf ausmif: odkh oGm: onf` maung hla. wang: sany kyaung: sui. swa: sany Maung Hla Win NomTop school Ob go NomSf ‘Maung Hla Win went to school.’ b. \ref mran as a doubled verb of manner armifvS0if: onf ausmif: odkh \ref \ref oGm: onf` maung hla. wang: sany kyaung: sui. mran mran swa: sany Maung Hla Win NomTop school Ob fast fast go NomSf ‘Maung Hla Win quickly went to school.’ c. \ref mran as a nominal complement of manner armifvS0if: onf ausmif: odkh t\ref oGm: onf` maung hla. wang: sany kyaung: sui. a-mran swa: sany Maung Hla Win NomTop school Ob fast go NomSf ‘Maung Hla Win quickly went to school.’ [Literally: MHW goes fast to school.] d. \ref mran not as a verb chain, or sequence ungrammatical armifvS0if: onf ausmif: odkh \ref oGm: onf` maung hla. wang: sany kyaung: sui. mran swa: sany Maung Hla Win NomTop school Ob fast go NomSf ‘Maung Hla Win quickly went to school.’ e. \ref mran as the main verb, swa: as subordinate clause armifvS0if: onf ausmif: oGm: aom tcg \ref onf` maung hla. wang: sany kyaung: swa: sau: a-hka mran sany Maung Hla Win NomTop school go Nom Nom-time fast NomSf ‘Maung Hla Win went quickly to school.’ [Literally: When MHW went to school, he quick.] The structural difference between 66a and 66b is the addition of a doubled verb of manner, \ref mran ‘fast’, which functions to qualify the action of the matrix verb ‘go’. It does this similarly to the compounding or doubling found in nominal forms. In 66b the “adverb” functions as the first pair of a compound—in this case, [VV] + V. The normal, balanced form of a compound is V + V or VV + VV. The first member or set of the doubled compound functions as a kind of qualifier or temporal antecedent to the final verb. This is due to the head-final nature of Burmese. The final matrix verb typically has greater scope and focus in the sentence as a whole, whereas the preceding verb or verb compound has a sense of prior temporality in a logical or chronological sequence in reference to the matrix verb, rather than to the sentence. The preceding verb generally has a less focal role in the overall action. Its sense of qualifying the verb tends to be a part of the semantics of the situation. It is also true that doubled verbs can appear as the main verb of the clause: juD:juD:bJ kri: kri: bhai: big + big of course ‘It’s really big’; \ynf: \ynf: aemhf prany: prany: nau: slow + slow ok? ‘Slowly, ok?’ They also appear in the imperative mood: \ref\refvdkuf mran mran luik fast + fast Rev ‘Get out of here fast’ This data is Colloquial Burmese since such use of stative verbs is less formal. The structure of the “adverb” as verb is displayed in figure 19. Figure 19. Adverb analyzed as a verbal compound construction. Stative verbs are semantically restricted to manner readings and cannot combine as a verb sequence except when reduplicated; hence, 66d is ungrammatical. There are also typical frozen expressions used in the preverbal position, such as t\rJwrf: a-mrai tam: N + V: always + continue = ‘always’. The nominalized form of the adverbial is accounted for by compounding with the previous nominal N + N or as a reduced clause N + V. The interpretation as derived nominals is supported by the fact that these forms often retain the ability to take complements: Ak[l:aehtvGwf csdef: buddhahu: ne. a-lut hkyin: Wednesday + avoidance + make an appointment ‘make an appointment avoiding Wednesday’; 0t0 pm: wa. a-wa. ca: be complete + Nom-complete + eat ‘eat until full’. The interpretation of the N + V constituency of the “adverbial” with the nominal rather than with the verb occurs when the verb is not reduplicated: vufawGhoif lak twe. sang [hand + meet] N + study ‘learn by experience’; a|vkH\ykwf re lum prut [water + be covered] N + boil ‘boil covered with water’; of\yefnm\yef|dkuf bhai pran nya pran ruik [ [right+turn] N + [left+ turn] N ] N + hit ‘hit with the left and then the right in turn’. The N + N interpretation is represented in figure 20. Figure 20. Adverb as nominal compound construction The analysis presented in 3.5.2 and 3.5.3 demonstrates how it may be possible to reduce the number of basic word categories. The constructions establish a framework from which the semantic component interprets both adjectival and adverbial senses, though this is more a problem for westerners than for native speakers of Burmese. The intuition of numerous linguists that underlyingly only Nouns and Verbs structure Burmese grammar has been shown to be plausible. It has also been shown that it is the structural rules that contribute the distinctive sense or usage of a particular nominal or verbal as it is constructed within the rules of grammar and that the same principles that structure grammar may contribute to the structure of semantic meaning. In the following sections we shall see that the same principles also structure larger grammatical units. Burmese grammar is structured by a common set of underlying units that combine iteratively in a regular system throughout the grammar. The functional load of grammar is reduced to a few basic patterns and constructions, shifting the bulk of the complexity to semantic and conceptual processes.

3.5.4 Types of Particles