1
st
Syllable Environment
2
nd
Syllable Environment
Examples
7. Stress Reduction
1
st
Syllable 2
nd
Syllable
Open Juncture Stressed at
head of phrase
with all tones except Plain
Stressed at head of
phrase
aumif:
kau ‘good’
aumif:onf
kau i ‘It’s good’
Close Juncture Stressed, with all tones but
Plain tone Reduced
stress
aumif:aumif:
kau au ‘very good’
Table 4. Summary of phonological processes associated with juncture
2.4.2 Semantic versus Grammatical Constituent Structure
The contrast between semantic analysis and grammatical analysis can best be seen in the compound nominal construction. Nominal compounds are often reanalyzed semantically as
nested attributes closest to the head nominal. The attributive modifier immediately preceding the head is often categorized as “one thing” semantically—that is, the immediate constituents
are reanalyzed as a conceptual blend. In this type of relation the constituent structure semantically would appear as in 9, which is graphically displayed in figure 14.
9 [Modifier + [Modifier + Head]
H
]
NP
Figure 14. Semantic constituency of blended heads
The internally nested modifier NP in figure 14 becomes head of the second modified NP. Often the justification of this type of constituency is social with pragmatically dominant
use of the NP as a semantic whole or unit, thus giving the phrase a sense of immediacy of constituency. Semantically, the resulting constituency is left-branching.
Contrastively, grammatical constituency is mostly right-branching with modifiers grouped as a series in their grammatical role of modifiers in a series in relation to each other
first and then to the head, as displayed in 10 and in figure 15. More detailed explanation of this process is illustrated in section 5.2.2.
10 [[Modifier + Modifier]
M
+ Head] ]
NP
Figure 15. Grammatical constituency of blended heads
The two types of analysis are displayed in 11 where the first grammatical reading is shown as ‘first mark’ and then the subsequent nominal forces the grammatical reading ‘first
marked post’. Semantic processing on the grammatical structure produces a semantic blend of ‘marked post’ to ‘bus stop’ as a single lexical compound noun. The first reading is entirely
grammatical but is semantically less astute since the most common understanding would be to interpret this sequence as ‘bus stop’. One might say there is semantic ambiguity between the
purely grammatical and the semantically blended heads.
11
yxr rSwf wdkif
pa.hta.ma. hmat tuing
‘first’ ‘mark’ ‘post’
‘first bus stop’ [ M H ]
[ M H ] Grammatical
Constituency ‘first marked post’
[ M H ] [ M H ]
Semantic Constituency
‘first [bus stop]’ At the clause level, U Pe Maung Tin 1956:194 called this type of semantic
constituency “noun-verbs.” The meaning of these verbs is “enlarged by an immediately preceding noun which has lost its nominative suffix.” Okell 1969 called these “tied nouns.”
This process can occur with either a grammatical subject, object, or an oblique, so it is not the predicate object and verb of traditional grammar. For this semantic process to occur, it is
necessary that no intervening postpositional particle occur on the nominal unit. Should one be used, the unity of the construction is broken and a different semantic interpretation results, as
in 12 and 13.
12
armifb acgif: udkuf onf
maung ba hkaung: kuik
sany
[ Maung Ba [head hurt ] ] NomSf
‘Maung Ba’s headaches.’ U Pe Maung Tin parses an example such as 12 not as ‘X’s head aches’ but as ‘X’s
headache’. Thus, ‘Maung Ba’s headache’ is close to the meaning of the Burmese, where the nominal “head” is incorporated into the meaning of the verb. This is the semantic
interpretation; it is a process of cognitive blending that occurs where the conditions are ripe due to juxtapositioning with no intervening postpositional particle. If the grammatical
objectpatient postposition occurs, the interpretation must be different.
13
armifb acgif: udk udkuf onf
maung ba hkaung: kui
kuik sany
[ [ [ Maung Ba head ] Ob ] hurt ] NomSf
‘Maung Ba’s head aches.’ The presence of the postposition forces in 13 the grammatical reading to be the
semantic reading. That is, it objectifies the grammatical object by creating an explicit object “head” with
udk
kui. and configures that object as the focus in 13. The possibility of
semantic merger in 12 is actually reinforced pragmatically by the social use of the term
acgif:udkufonf
hkaung: kui kuik sany ‘headaches’ as a commonly used response since
the subject is often unnecessary for explicit mention. This type of noun-verb pragmatic extension or implicature often acquires a new meaning greater than the sum of its parts:
a|aomuf
re sauk ‘water drink’ means ‘drink’;
jurf:ydkxdk:
kram: pui htui: ‘bug bites’
means to ‘loaf around’ literally ‘bed bug bites’, with an underlying implication of sleeping around.
2.5 Functional Types of Nominalization