Equative and attributive sentences

and one of the noun phrases, generally the first, 34 consists of a free relative clause. It could be argued that such a clause has a true relative clause construction with an empty head position, since its head would, in all cases, be identical to the second NP of the sentence. 186 a. Kydy-tho to. heavy-WH.SUBJ it ‘It is heavy.’ b. Kydy-tho to siba. heavy-WH.SUBJ the stone ‘The stone is heavy.’ c. Firo-thi li wadili. big-WH.SUBJ the man ‘The man is big.’ d. Wakhai-tho kho to hala. bad-WH.SUBJ not the bench ‘The bench is very good.’ Although several of the above attributive sentences contain the form to, this to is the article ‘the’ and is not the same as the copular verb to found in equative sentences. Unlike the copular verb, the article must agree with the gender and number of the head of the noun phrase, i.e. to ‘the non-masculine’, li ‘the masculine singular’ na ‘the human plu- ral’. Furthermore, equative sentences are divided into three intonational units, with poten- tial hesitation points before and after the copula. Attributive sentences are usually spoken with a single intonation contour, and one cannot pause between the article and the rest of the noun phrase.

3.2.3.2 Stative sentences with stative verbs

Stative sentences can also be constructed with a stative verb as their main verb see Section 2.4.2 on stative verbs followed by a subject noun phrase. 187. Alekhebe-ka li wadili. happy-INDIC the man ‘The man iswas happy.’ 3.2 Sentence Structure 79 34 I have recorded several instances where the order of the NPs is reversed. However, in these cases the first NP seems to behave as if it is in the TOPIC position of the sentence. It has to be old information, and is usually separated intonationally and rhythmically from the rest. Furthermore, when editing their own texts, Arawaks often add a sentence final pronoun to such sentences, making the last part of the sentence, again, into an attributive sentence starting with a relative clause and ending with an NP. To siba, kydy-tho . the stone heavy-WH.SUBJ ‘The stone is heavy.’ ‘Concerning the stone, it is heavy.’ To siba, kydy-tho to . the stone heavy-WH-SUBJ it ‘Concerning the stone, it is heavy.’ These sentences differ both from the other stative sentences and from event sentences. Un- like other stative sentences which have, at most, a bare copula for a verb, the verb in stative verb sentences can receive a rather full set of tense, affix, and pronoun affixes. 35 188 a. Jara-ka bo. here-INDIC you ‘You are here.’ b. Hebe-fa to dalhidi-koana. full-FUT the run-thing ‘The carbus will be full.’ c. Seme-ka to sikalho. sweet-INDIC the sugarcane ‘The sugarcane is sweet.’ d. Somole-ka-i hibin. drunk-INDIC-he already ‘He is already drunk.’ As can be seen in the above examples, this type of sentence also differs from other types in its word order. This is the only type of sentence in Arawak where the subject clearly seems to follow the verb. 36 189 a. Stative sentence: Fonasia-fa lirabo. hungry-FUT he.over.there ‘He will be hungry.’ b. Simple intransitive event sentence: Lirabo osy-fa. he.over.there go-FUT ‘He will go.’ In addition to the basic verb-subject ordering for these stative sentences, one occasion- ally does find subject-first ones. However, such sentences seem to be the result of subject fronting. As is the case with event sentences, in stative sentences it is also possible to front various constituents to the LSAP position. 80 Noun Phrase and Sentence Syntax 35 See Figure 8 in Section 2.4.2.1 for a summary of stative verb affixes. 36 Semantically, the second NP in attributive sentences seems more subject-like than the first. However, there is always some uncertainty created by the fact that the first constituent in attributive sentences seems to be an NP also, and therefore might be the subject.