Event Verb Structure Event Verbs
At the peripheries of the event verb word order classes –1 and +8 are the personal pronoun prefixes and suffixes which were previously discussed. A morphologically bound
pronoun occurring in position –1 of an event verb is the subject of that verb; one occurring in position +8 is the direct object of course intransitive verbs cannot take a direct ob-
ject.
39
Order classes +1 to +7 consist of various aspect, mood, and directional suffixes. A brief discussion of each order class and the suffixes in it follows.
2.4.1.3.1 Subordinator Class +1 In the first position, +1, following the stem, only the suffix -n occurs. It is the subordi-
nating morpheme SUB, used whenever a verb is in a subordinate clause, and can co-occur with any of the other suffixes. For example:
55 To d-osy-n-bo
jon, abali
anda-the ... the
I-go-SUB-CONT there
one.man arrive-COME
‘Concerning what happened while I was going there, a man arrived ...’ The suffix -n, if it is the only affix on a verb, yields the verb’s citation form—that is, the
form used by Arawaks to talk about a particular verb. It is roughly equivalent to an English gerund or infinitive. For example:
56 andy-n ‘to arrivearriving’
anda-n ‘to touchtouching’
osy-n ‘to gogoing’
salabady-n ‘to make a square post from a log’
2.4.1.3.2 Relativizers Class +2 The second position after the verb stem consists of three relativizing suffixes -thi, -tho,
and -sia, one of which must be used whenever an event verb’s subject or direct object has been relativized.
57 a. -thi ‘WH.SUBJ’ relativized [+human +male] subject: Li
wadili [› dykha-THI-bo d-arakabosan khonan] andy-fa
th-abo mothi. The man [› look.after-WH.SUBJ-CONT my-gun
on] arrive-FUT it-with tomorrow
‘The man who is looking after my gun will arrive with it tomorrow.’ b. -tho ‘WH.SUBJ’ relativized [+human –male] or [–human] subject
Da-wada-bo to
pero [›
rydy-THO de].
I-search-CONT the
dog [›
bite-WH.SUBJ me]
‘I am looking for the dog that bit me.’ c. -sia ‘WH.OBJ’ relativized direct object
D-othika to
kharasy-n [by-mikodo-SIA
› da-myn.]
I-receive the
write-SUB [you-send-WH.OBJ
› me-BEN]
‘I received the writing =letter you sent me.’ 32
Phonology and Morphology
39
See Section 2.3.1 for further discussion on pronouns.
The relativization suffixes are discussed in some detail in other sections of this study see Chapter 4 on relative clauses, and consequently are not dealt with further here.
2.4.1.3.3 Expectation Class +3 The third order position after the stem consists of two suffixes having to do with expectancy:
58 -ra ‘EXPECT’ expected action or result
-na ‘UNEXP’
unexpected action or result The suffix -ra added to the verb indicates that the action or event described is somehow expected
because of the preceding linguistic context or the situational context. Thus, a man who had previously asked if he could go to a party with a neighbor meets that neighbor again when that neighbor stops by
to pick him up. The neighbor asks the man whether he is going to the party, and the man answers:
59 D-osy-ra-fa. I-go-EXPECT-FUT
‘Of course I will go’. The suffix -na is the opposite of -ra. It indicates that the action or event is unexpected. Thus,
in a story about playing with an unexploded bomb that later explodes, the story teller says: 60 W-etha-na
kho oda-ha khona to
wa-birabirady-n-bo. we-know-UNEXP not dead-NGEN on
the we-play.around-SUB-CONT
‘We didn’t know but should have we were playing around with death.’ 2.4.1.3.4 Modality Class +4
The fourth order class after the stem consists of three suffixes which have to do with de- sire, potentiality or ability, and necessity:
61 -ma ‘HABIL’
habilitative -thi
‘DESID’ desiderative
-li ‘NESS’
necessitative The suffix -ma ‘habilitative’ is used to express potentiality in the sense of either ability or
permission. Thus, 62 Tho-khota-ma no.
she-eat-HABIL it ‘She can eat it.’
is similar to its English gloss and can mean either ‘she is able to eat it’ or ‘she is allowed to eat it’. In contexts where the distinction between the two is important and only the ‘ability’
interpretation is desired, the verb wadilin ‘to be able to’ is used. The suffix -thi ‘desiderative’ indicates desire or intention.
2.4 Verbs 33
63 B-osa-thi-ka. you-go-DESID-PERF
‘Do you wantintend to go?’ This suffix is never found without one of the tenseaspect suffixes in the next order class.
The suffix -li ‘necessitative’ indicates obligation or necessity. This suffix not only does not co-occur with the habilitative or desiderative suffixes but also does not co-occur with the
tenseaspect suffixes of the next class, +5. This may be because the meaning of this suffix seems to have a temporally forward-looking component.
64 By-malhikhotoa-li no.
you-learn-NESS it
‘You should learn it.’ The suffix -li leaves some doubt as to the outcome of the situation. If certainty of the out-
come as in the English ‘must’ needs to be expressed, the future suffix -fa must be used see the discussion below.
2.4.1.3.5 TenseAspect Class +5 The fifth position after the stem consists of five tenseaspect suffixes. In addition, the ab-
sence of all of these suffixes has its own meaning, normally past. However, the past tense interpretation, when none of the five tenseaspect suffixes is present, does not occur when
-li ‘necessitative’ or -ma ‘habilitative’ are present.
65 -bo ‘CONT’
present continuative -ka
‘PERF’ perfect
-fa ‘FUT’
future -ja
‘PAST.CONT’ past continuative
-bia ‘INCH’
inchoative -›
‘PAST’ simple past
The suffixes in this class are analyzed in more detail in Chapter 5 and consequently are dealt with only briefly here. The meanings in 65 for the suffixes apply to their use in isolated utter-
ances. They all represent combinations of tense and aspect, and the tense component of their meanings is actually what Comrie 1985 calls “relative tense” see Chapter 5. This implies
that their temporal reference is affected by the discourse context in which they are found.
The suffix -bo ‘present continuative’ indicates a continuing or progressing action— normally in the present.
66 a. Ama b-ani-bo?
what you-do-CONT
‘What are you doing?’ b. Ly-dalhida-ra-bo-the.
he-run-EXPECT-CONT-BACK ‘He is running back of course.’
34 Phonology and Morphology
The suffix -ka ‘perfect’ is used to indicate a completed action where the effects of that ac- tion extend into the present. For example, if one says
67 Da-sokosa-ka. I-wash.clothes-PERF
‘I have washed the clothes.’ then the clothes have been washed and are now clean. Similarly, the question
68 By-ka-ka? you-bathe-PERF
‘Did you bathe?’ or ‘Have you bathed?’ not only asks whether a bath has been taken, but also asks whether its effects are still pres-
ent. Thus the question could not be answered in the affirmative if one has bathed but is now dirty again.
The future suffix -fa -ha in some dialects generally indicates future certainty. 69 a. L-osy-fa.
he-go-FUT ‘He will go.’
b. Lo-khota-thi-fa. he-eat-DESID-FUT
‘He will want to eat.’ When this suffix is used in an imperative sentence,
40
it is best translated by the English word ‘must.’
70 B-osy-fa you-go-FUT
‘You must go’ The suffix -ja ‘past continuative’ is used to express events which had a duration in the
past. The action expressed by the verb is not on-going in the present and the hearer does not know if the event ran to completion.
71 Tho-boka-ja. she-cook-PAST.CONT
‘She was cooking a meal.’ The suffix -nbia ‘inchoative’ generally indicates that a change of state has begun or will
begin. 72 To
ada iwi
bikhidi-nbia. the
tree fruit
grow-INCH ‘The fruit is ripening.’
2.4 Verbs 35
40
Imperative sentences can occur only with second-person singular and plural subjects, and with first-person plural ones. They are distinguished from corresponding indicative sentences solely by their intonation.
This suffix is also used where ‘in order to’ would be used in English. 73 Li-sika-bo
to polata da-myn
da-jonta-bia amathali
ly-myn. he-give-CONT the
money me-BEN I-buy-INCH
something him-BEN
‘He gave me the money to buy something for him.’ When the inchoative is added to the basic-stem form of a verb, which is the case in ex-
ample 72, it always has the form -nbia. When it is used on an a-stem, as in 73, the n is often dropped, and the suffix is just -bia. The precise explanation for the dropping of the n
has not been determined. If a verb in an isolated utterance does not have one of the five aspecttense suffixes men-
tioned in this section, nor -li ‘necessitative’ nor -ma ‘habilitative’, it is always taken to be equivalent to the simple past tense.
74 a. D-osa. I-go
‘I went.’ b. Da-fara
aba dodole.
I-kill one
peccary. ‘I killed a peccary.’
c. Miaka d-osa
forto-nro. yesterday
I-go town-to
‘Yesterday I went to town.’ As might be expected if the lack of a tenseaspect suffix is a “significant zero” with a past
tense meaning, this -› is unacceptable with a future time adverbial. 75 Mothi
d-osa forto-nro.
tomorrow I-go
town-to ‘Tomorrow I went to town.’
2.4.1.3.6 Directionals Classes +6 and +7 The sixth and seventh order positions after the stem consist of two directional suffixes:
76 -ba ‘AWAY’
motion away, going -the
‘BACK’ motion back, coming
The suffix -ba in the +6 position indicates that the action of the verb involves physical or psychological motion away from the speaker’s reference point i.e. away from the
speaker at the moment of the utterance, or away from his habitual place or state, or away from where he should be. Thus,
77 L-osa-ba he-go-AWAY
‘He went away.’ 36
Phonology and Morphology
The suffix -the in the +7 position is the converse of the preceding suffix and therefore indicates physical or psychological motion toward the speaker’s reference point:
78 B-anda-li-the d-amyn.
you-arrive-NESS-BACK me-by ‘You must come to me e.g. at my house’.
As might be inferred from the fact that the directional suffixes are listed in different or- der classes, these suffixes can co-occur, and when they co-occur, they do so in a specific or-
der: -ba-the. Thus, it is possible to say: 79 L-osa-ba-the.
he-go-AWAY-BACK ‘He went and came back.’