Stative sentences with stative verbs
These sentences differ both from the other stative sentences and from event sentences. Un- like other stative sentences which have, at most, a bare copula for a verb, the verb in
stative verb sentences can receive a rather full set of tense, affix, and pronoun affixes.
35
188 a. Jara-ka bo.
here-INDIC you
‘You are here.’ b. Hebe-fa
to dalhidi-koana.
full-FUT the
run-thing ‘The carbus will be full.’
c. Seme-ka to
sikalho. sweet-INDIC
the sugarcane
‘The sugarcane is sweet.’ d. Somole-ka-i
hibin. drunk-INDIC-he
already ‘He is already drunk.’
As can be seen in the above examples, this type of sentence also differs from other types in its word order. This is the only type of sentence in Arawak where the subject clearly
seems to follow the verb.
36
189 a. Stative sentence: Fonasia-fa
lirabo. hungry-FUT
he.over.there ‘He will be hungry.’
b. Simple intransitive event sentence: Lirabo
osy-fa. he.over.there
go-FUT ‘He will go.’
In addition to the basic verb-subject ordering for these stative sentences, one occasion- ally does find subject-first ones. However, such sentences seem to be the result of subject
fronting. As is the case with event sentences, in stative sentences it is also possible to front various constituents to the LSAP position.
80 Noun Phrase and Sentence Syntax
35
See Figure 8 in Section 2.4.2.1 for a summary of stative verb affixes.
36
Semantically, the second NP in attributive sentences seems more subject-like than the first. However, there is always some uncertainty created by the fact that the first constituent in attributive sentences seems to be an NP
also, and therefore might be the subject.
In event sentences, I have no clear examples of a subject, other than a relative pronoun or question word, moving to the LSAP position.
37
This may simply be because subject posi- tion is adjacent to the LSAP and therefore such movement is undetectable.
In stative verb sentences, however, the subject can be fronted to LSAP, and the results of this fronting are visible. The fronting creates stative verb sentences with what appears to
be a surface subject-verb constituent order. That this subject has landed in the LSAP posi- tion can be shown by the fact that in stative sentences with fronted subjects, no other con-
stituents can be fronted to the LSAP.
190 a. Hebe-fa to
dalhidi-koana mothi.
full-FUT the
run-thing tomorrow
‘The car will be full tomorrow.’ b. Mothi
hebe-fa to
dalhidi-koana. tomorrow
full-FUT the
run-thing ‘Tomorrow the car will be full.
c. To dalhidi-koana
hebe-fa mothi.
the run-thing
full-FUT tomorrow
‘The car will be full tomorrow.’ d. Mothi
to dalhidi-koana
hebe-fa. tomorrow
the run-thing
full-FUT ‘Tomorrow the car will be full.’
That the nominal following stative verbs is, in fact, the subject can be shown by the fact that when it is relativized, the subject-relativization suffix -thi-tho WH.SUBJ appears.
191 a. aba hebe-tho
kekere a
full-WH.SUBJ basket
‘a full basket a basket which is full’ b. to
hehe-tho ada-yda
the yellow-WH.SUBJ
tree-skin ‘the yellow bark’
It is possible to use postpositions in Arawak as stative verbs see also Section 2.4.2.2 on derived stative verbs by simply placing them in the verb position in the clause. When
used in this way, they can receive tense and aspect suffixes as well as the pronominal pre- fixes and suffixes normally used with stative verbs. Interestingly, sentences of this type
have two NP arguments and could be regarded as stative analogues to transitive event sen- tences. As in other stative sentences, the subject in these sentences also follows the verb.
3.2 Sentence Structure 81
37
That a question word or relative pronoun is in the LSAP position can be shown by the fact that it cannot be preceded by another fronted constituent unless that constituent is in TOPIC position. Thus the following
sentence, spoken without an intonation break between ‘tomorrow’ and the rest of the sentence, is unacceptable: Mothi
alikan fonasia-fa
? tomorrow
who hungry-FUT
‘Tomorrow who will be hungry?’
192 a. Mothi bahy
loko-fa no.
tomorrow house
in-FUT she
‘Tomorrow she will be in the house.’ b. Lo-jono
koboroko-ka da-mathia.
his-extended.family among-INDIC
my-friend ‘My friend is e.g. sitting there with his family.’
c. Tho-loko-ka-i. it-in-INDIC-he
‘He is in it e.g. the house.’ Again, the grammatical roles of the nominals in question can be identified through the
test of relativization. Arawak seems to treat the noun phrase preceding such a postpositional verb as an object, and the one following as a subject. As is the case with
both event and stative clauses, when the subject, in this case the nominal following the verb, is relativized, the subject relativization suffix -tho-thi WH.SUBJ appears.
193 a. Bahy oko-ka
li wadili.
house in-INDIC
the man
‘The man is in the house.’ b. li
wadili bahy
loko-thi the
man house
in-WH.SUBJ ‘the man in the house’
c. Li wadili [da-sikoa
loko-thi koba] anda.
the man
[my-house in-WH.SUBJ
past] arrive
‘The man, who used to be in my house, came.’ When the nominal preceding the verb in these sentences is relativized, it is treated the
same as an object of transitive event sentence, and the object-relativization suffix -sia WH.OBJ appears see also Chapter 4 on relative clauses.
194 to
sikoa ama
loko-sia-ka li
wadili… the
house what in-WH.OBJ-INDIC
the man…
‘the house in which the man iswas…’ 82
Noun Phrase and Sentence Syntax